JoVE Logo

Zaloguj się

Aby wyświetlić tę treść, wymagana jest subskrypcja JoVE. Zaloguj się lub rozpocznij bezpłatny okres próbny.

W tym Artykule

  • Podsumowanie
  • Streszczenie
  • Wprowadzenie
  • Protokół
  • Wyniki
  • Dyskusje
  • Ujawnienia
  • Podziękowania
  • Materiały
  • Odniesienia
  • Przedruki i uprawnienia

Podsumowanie

We present a simple and unbiased olfactory test in mice. With this protocol olfactory discrimination, preference, avoidance and sensitivity to a novel odor as compared to water can be assessed in single behavioral sessions. This method is indicated for a single experimenter and analysis is based on computer-assisted video processing.

Streszczenie

Olfaction is highly conserved among species and is required for reproduction and survival.

In humans, olfaction is also one of the senses that is affected with aging and is a strong predictor of neurodegenerative diseases. Thus, olfaction testing is used as a non-invasive diagnostic method to detect neurological deficits early on. In order to understand the mechanisms underlying olfactory network susceptibility, olfactory research in rodents has gained momentum in the past decade.

Here, we present a very simple, time efficient and reproducible olfactory testing method of innate odor perception and sensitivity in mice without the need of any prior food or water restriction. The tests are performed in a familiar environment to the mice, require only the scents and a 2 min session of odorant exposure. The analysis is performed, post-hoc, using computer-assisted commands on ImageJ and can be, therefore, carried out from start to end by one researcher.

This protocol does not require any special hardware or setup and is indicated for any laboratory interested in testing olfactory perception and sensitivity.

Wprowadzenie

Olfaction is one of the most developed and important sensory functions in mammals. Any impairment in olfactory activity may affect food intake, social behavior and, in the worst case scenario, even survival. In humans, olfactory deterioration is age dependent1 and is considered a strong predictor of neurological disorders26. The olfactory identification test developed by the University of Pennsylvania currently represents one of the most used, non-invasive and quantifiable, diagnostic tests which can assess early neurological deficits7 and predict with high probability the progression of dementia8,9.

The accessibility of the olfactory system and the prominence of olfaction in rodents, has sparked an intense line of research addressing the mechanisms underlying olfactory functions10. We have previously shown that loss of function of the signaling receptor Notch1 affects olfactory avoidance11. In this protocol we use mice lacking the signaling ligand, Jagged1, in neurons or glia to study olfactory performance.

Innate olfaction is defined by three parameters as perception, discrimination between odors and olfactory sensitivity4. Olfactory testing in rodents can be done in a variety of ways and some behavioral studies make use of olfactometers, which provide the odor to the animal at a specific vapor concentrations and in a precise time frame1214. Nevertheless, this instrumentation is expensive and may be available only in specialized facilities. In our work, we provide a simple, fast and reproducible olfactory testing protocol, which is carried out using volatile scents. The tests described measure perception to an attractant or a repellent odor and evaluate the discrimination between the scent and water11,15,16. Using the same setup, we also can measure the sensitivity to an odor at different concentrations16,17. The post-hoc computer-assisted video processing, inspired by the work of Page and colleagues18, provides unbiased results without the need of experimental blinding and allowing for a single person to carry out the whole experiment.

This protocol is intended to provide a starting point for studying olfactory behavior in mice.

Protokół

All the animal procedures are in accordance with the EU Directive 2010/63/EU on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes and are approved by the local Animal Care Committee (Canton of Fribourg, Switzerland).

1. Animal Preparation

  1. Experimental animals
    1. Perform experiments on adult male wild type and transgenic mice (C57BL/6 background) of 3-5 months of age. The three groups of mice correspond to wild type littermate controls (group A, Jagged1 flox/flox19) and two conditional KO mouse lines (group B, Jagged1ncKO and C, Jagged1gcKO).
    2. House mice under standard laboratory conditions in a ventilated room, with a 12 hr controlled dark/light cycle and provide food and water ad libitum.

2. Experimental Setup

  1. Experimental arena
    1. For the experimental arena, use a clean sterilized mouse cage (36 cm length x 20.5 cm width x 13.5 cm height) (Figure 1A).
    2. Assign each mouse to a numbered cage with fresh bedding, 3 cm high. If cages are reused, as in the odor sensitivity test, take the following measures to avoid cross-contamination between odors and mice.
      1. Mark the water side.
      2. Clean the narrower walls of the cages with two tissue papers sprayed with 70% ethanol, one for each side.
      3. Pile up cages according to the genotype of the mice and store temporarily under a laminar hood.
  2. Camera
    1. Mount a camera on a customized tripod with the objective at 58 cm from the bottom of the cage (Figure 1A). Fix the position of the Tripod and the cage and delimit with marks to allow for the camera to be centered on the top of the cage.
    2. Record videos at 320 pixels x 240 pixels, 15.08 frames per second as MOV files.
  3. Odors
    1. Resuspend the scents, when indicated, in the solvent in which they are soluble.
    2. For the preference test use peanut butter. Resuspend the peanut butter in peanut oil (10% w/v).
    3. For the avoidance test use pure 2-Methylbutyric (2-MB) acid (98%).
    4. For the sensitivity test, use female urine from the same mouse colony and background (C57BL/6).
      1. For convenience collect the urine 1-2 days prior to the olfactory test. Restrain and hold the mouse under the hood with its belly above the cage grid. Under the cage grid place a plastic petri dish to collect the drops of urine.
      2. Collect the urine from each female in a 1.5 ml tube and mix all the urine samples to normalize for variability between animals. Store at -20 °C until use.
      3. On the day of the experiment, thaw the urine and perform 4 dilutions in double distilled water at a dilution factor of 10 (1:10, 1:100; 1:1,000; 1:10,000).

3. Olfactory Testing

Note: In this protocol odors have been deliberately chosen which are perceived as strong attractants (peanut butter and female urine) or strong repellent (2-MB acid)15. It is important to carry out the preference and sensitivity tests to pleasant odors prior to the avoidance test to eliminate the possibility of any interference with the olfactory behavior. Nevertheless, for the sake of simplicity, in this paper, preference and avoidance test will be both described under the perception test. Each behavioral session starts with a habituation phase.

  1. Habituation Phase
    1. Place the animal in the clean assigned cage and let it explore for 5 min (Figure 1B). Since the environment of the experimental cage is familiar to the home cage, this short time is enough to allow for habituation.
    2. If the sensitivity test is completed in one day, perform habituation only once before the application of the highest diluted odor. If the sensitivity test is carried out on different days, on each day a habituation phase on a new clean cage is needed.
  2. Perception Test
    1. After habituation, activate the camera and immediately pipette 60 μl of the pleasant scent (peanut butter) and 60 μl of the neutral scent (tap water) onto the opposite walls of the cage at about 10 cm from the bottom (Figure 1C).
    2. Let the mouse explore the odors for 2 min (Figure 1D). Thereafter, switch off the camera.
    3. At this point, proceed with the next mouse starting from the habituation phase. Perform the avoidance test exactly in the same way by applying 60 μl of the repellent odor (2-MB acid) and 60 μl of water.
  3. Sensitivity test
    1. Evaluate the attraction threshold of male mice to increasing concentrations of female urine in the following order: 1:10,000; 1:1,000; 1:100; 1:10 and pure urine.
    2. After habituation, expose each mouse to the highest dilution pipetted by the experimenter as previously described in 3.2.1.
    3. Record the exploratory behavior of urine versus water, within a 2 min time frame on a video camera. After all mice cohorts are tested for the highest dilution (1:10,000), expose to a higher concentration of urine, as indicated above.

4. Post-hoc Data Analysis

Note: All behavioral tests described are processed post hoc following the data analysis instructions.

  1. Open MOV Files in ImageJ for Windows systems
    1. Install Quick Time for Java using the customized settings from http://www.apple.com/quicktime/download .
    2. Install the Quick Time plugin from the ImageJ website (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/plugins/qt-capture.html).
    3. Import the QTJava.zip (C:\Program Files\QuickTime\QTSystem) into the library extension of ImageJ (.ImageJ\jre\lib\ext).
    4. Copy also the QTJava.zip in the plugins folder and rename it as QTJava.jar.
    5. Install the six scripts attached in the Macros folder (ImageJ\plugins\Macros).
    6. Open ImageJ and compile and run the Quick Time plugin, thereafter close ImageJ.
    7. Reopen ImageJ and open the MOV file using File> import> using Quick Time.
  2. Video Adjustment
    1. Once the video file is opened in ImageJ, cut the video in order to obtain a constant 2 min exploration from the time the experimenter has pipetted the odorants into the cage (T0). Identify the frame corresponding to the T0 and remove the previous frames using increments of 1 (ImageJ\Image\Stacks\ToolsSlice remover). Use the same command to delete all the frames exceeding the 2 min exploration.
    2. Make sure that the cage is centered and if necessary use the Image>Transform>Rotate command to align it.
  3. Video Processing
    Note: Video processing is fully computer-assisted and uses macros commands accompanying this paper.
    1. In order to restrict the area onto the cage of a 127 pixels x 218 pixels size run the Step 1 macro from the Plugin>Macros>Run command. Move the fixed rectangle over the cage (Figure 2, Step 1).
    2. Crop the area of the cage on the region of interest (ROI) using the Step 2 macro (Figure 2, Step 2).
    3. Use the Step 3 macro to extract the mouse image from the background by assigning a threshold signal, despeckling and filtering the signal variance. The output values in the Z axis plot indicate the mean grey values, corresponding to the intensity of the mouse shadow moving within the ROI of the “water chamber” during the 2 min exploration. Copy the results into a worksheet named according to the ROI in a spreadsheet file (Figure 2, Step 3).
    4. Use the Step 4 macro to extract the mean grey values of the mouse in the ROI “odor chamber”. Copy the results into a worksheet named according to the ROI in the same spreadsheet file as in 4.3.3 (Figure 2, Step 4).
    5. In order to further restrict the analysis of the mouse movement in the ROI “water perimeter” use the Step 5 macro. Copy the result onto the worksheet named according to the ROI in the same spreadsheet file as in 4.3.3 (Figure 2, Step 5).
    6. To restrict the analysis of the mouse movement in the ROI “odor perimeter” use the Step 6 macro. Copy the result onto the worksheet named according to the ROI spreadsheet file as in 4.3.3 (Figure 2, Step 6).
    7. Process all videos and check for consistency in the number of frames per animals. Here, record all animals for 1,810 frames corresponding to a 2 min exploration session.
    8. For each animal and for each ROI sort frames with mean grey values larger than 0. Divide the number of frames by the values corresponding to 1 second and obtain the seconds spent in each ROI.

5. Statistical Analysis

  1. For each test verify homogeneity of variance within groups/ genotypes by Bartlett’s test using the formula available on http://www.real-statistics.com/one-way-analysis-of-variance-anova/homogeneity-variances/ .
  2. In the attraction and avoidance test, carry out comparisons between the times spent with water versus odor within one group using a non-directional Student’s t-test assuming equal or unequal variances depending on the results of the Bartlett’s test. Compare the times spent with the odors subtracted by the time spent with water between the genotypes by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post-hoc test.
  3. In the sensitivity test analyze the comparisons of the time spent with the odor subtracted from the time spent with water among groups at a specific dilutions of urine by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post-hoc test. Compare the sensitivity among groups to growing odor concentrations by 2-Way ANOVA with repetitions with Bonferroni’s post-hoc test.
  4. Interaction between genotypes and treatments in the attraction and avoidance test are investigated by 2-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post-hoc test.

Wyniki

The perception test measures the attraction to peanut butter and avoidance to 2-MB acid. Three groups of mice are tested and the time spent in the “odor perimeter” are quantified as compared to water. In the preference test, the control group A displays significant preference to the odor as compared to water (t8 = 2.52, p <0.05). On the other hand, group B does not show any significant attraction to peanut butter and spends more time with water (t6 = 3.22, p <0.05). Thus, it beha...

Dyskusje

The tests proposed in this protocol allow to evaluate different aspects of innate olfactory behavior in mice: perception to odors, discrimination between odors versus water and sensitivity to odors. This protocol can be applied to any odor according to the preference and avoidance scale previously shown15. Since the protocol is based on exploratory activity it is important that mice do not display any motor impairment or anxiety which may affect their movement and interfere with olfactory exploration. The test...

Ujawnienia

There is no conflict of interest.

Podziękowania

This work is funded by the Swiss National Foundation (31_138429) and Synapsis Foundation for the support of research on Alzheimer’s disease.

Materiały

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
Mouse cageItalplast (Italy)1144B36 cm length x 20.5 cm width x 13.5 cm height
Chipped wood beddingAbedd (Austria)LTE E-0013 cm high
Peanut butterMigros (Switzerland)NA1:10
2-MethylbutyricSigma Aldrich (Switzerland)W269514Pure
Female urine from fertile females of same mouse strainNANADilution series
CameraOlympus (US)Camedia C-8080MOV files
Quicktime for Java (Windows)Apple (USA)NAvideo plugin for visualizing MOV files
ImageJ for WindowsNIH (USA)NAVideo Processing/Analysis

Odniesienia

  1. Doty, R. L., Kamath, V. The influences of age on olfaction: a review. Cognitive Science. 5, 20 (2014).
  2. Mesholam, R. I., Moberg, P. J., Mahr, R. N., Doty, R. L. Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Archives of Neurology. 55 (1), 84-90 (1998).
  3. Moberg, P. J., et al. Olfactory Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: A Qualitative and Quantitative Review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 21 (3), 325-340 (1999).
  4. Kovács, T. Mechanisms of olfactory dysfunction in aging and neurodegenerative disorders. Ageing Research Reviews. 3 (2), 215-232 (2004).
  5. Barrios, F. A., et al. Olfaction and neurodegeneration in HD. Neuroreport. 18 (1), 73-76 (2007).
  6. Doty, R. L. Olfaction in Parkinson’s disease and related disorders. Neurobiology of Disease. 46 (3), 527-552 (2012).
  7. Doty, R. L., Shaman, P., Dann, M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiology & Behavior. 32 (3), 489-502 (1984).
  8. Devanand, D. p., et al. Olfactory Deficits in Patients With Mild Cognitive Impairment Predict Alzheimer’s Disease at Follow-Up. American Journal of Psychiatry. 157 (9), 1399-1405 (2000).
  9. Conti, M. Z., et al. Odor Identification Deficit Predicts Clinical Conversion from Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia Due to Alzheimer’s Disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 28 (5), 391-399 (2013).
  10. Keller, A., Vosshall, L. B. Better Smelling Through Genetics: Mammalian Odor Perception. Current opinion in neurobiology. 18 (4), 364-369 (2008).
  11. Brai, E., et al. Notch1 activity in the olfactory bulb is odour-dependent and contributes to olfactory behaviour. European Journal of Neuroscience. 40 (10), 3436-3449 (2014).
  12. Larson, J., Hoffman, J. S., Guidotti, A., Costa, E. Olfactory discrimination learning deficit in heterozygous reeler mice. Brain Research. 971 (1), 40-46 (2003).
  13. Alonso, M., et al. Olfactory Discrimination Learning Increases the Survival of Adult-Born Neurons in the Olfactory Bulb. The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (41), 10508-10513 (2006).
  14. Wesson, D. W., Keller, M., Douhard, Q., Baum, M. J., Bakker, J. Enhanced urinary odor discrimination in female aromatase knockout (ArKO) mice. Hormones and behavior. 49 (5), 580-586 (2006).
  15. Kobayakawa, K., et al. Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature. 450 (7169), 503-508 (2007).
  16. Witt, R. M., Galligan, M. R., Despinoy, J., Segal, R. Olfactory Behavioral Testing in the Adult Mouse. Journal of Visualized Experiments JoVE. (23), (2009).
  17. Lee, A. W., Emsley, J. G., Brown, R. E., Hagg, T. Marked differences in olfactory sensitivity and apparent speed of forebrain neuroblast migration in three inbred strains of mice. Neuroscience. 118 (1), 263-270 (2003).
  18. Page, D. T., et al. Computerized assessment of social approach behavior in mouse. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 3, 48 (2009).
  19. Nyfeler, Y., et al. Jagged1 signals in the postnatal subventricular zone are required for neural stem cell self-renewal. Embo J. 24 (19), 3504-3515 (2005).
  20. Tong, M. T., Peace, S. T., Cleland, T. A. Properties and mechanisms of olfactory learning and memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8, (2014).
  21. Corthell, J., Stathopoulos, A., Watson, C., Bertram, R., Trombley, P. Olfactory Bulb Monoamine Concentrations Vary with Time of Day. Neuroscience. 247, 234-241 (2013).
  22. Lehmkuhl, A. M., Dirr, E. R., Fleming, S. M. Olfactory assays for mouse models of neurodegenerative disease. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE. (90), e51804 (2014).

Przedruki i uprawnienia

Zapytaj o uprawnienia na użycie tekstu lub obrazów z tego artykułu JoVE

Zapytaj o uprawnienia

Przeglądaj więcej artyków

Keywords OlfactionOlfactory TestingMiceRodentsOdorant ExposureInnate Odor PerceptionOlfactory SensitivityComputer assisted AnalysisImageJNeurodegenerative Diseases

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Prywatność

Warunki Korzystania

Zasady

Badania

Edukacja

O JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. Wszelkie prawa zastrzeżone