JoVE Logo

Zaloguj się

Aby wyświetlić tę treść, wymagana jest subskrypcja JoVE. Zaloguj się lub rozpocznij bezpłatny okres próbny.

W tym Artykule

  • Podsumowanie
  • Streszczenie
  • Wprowadzenie
  • Protokół
  • Wyniki
  • Dyskusje
  • Ujawnienia
  • Podziękowania
  • Materiały
  • Odniesienia
  • Przedruki i uprawnienia

Podsumowanie

A paradigm is presented to analyze the acquisition of a high-precision skilled forelimb reaching task in rats.

Streszczenie

Movements are the main measurable output of central nervous system function. Developing behavioral paradigms that allow detailed analysis of motor learning and execution is of critical importance in order to understand the principles and processes that underlie motor function. Here we present a paradigm to study movement acquisition within a daily session of training (within-session) representing the fast learning component and primary acquisition as well as skilled motor learning over several training sessions (between-session) representing the slow learning component and consolidation of the learned task. This behavioral paradigm increases the degree of difficulty and complexity of the motor skill task due to two features: First, the animal realigns its body prior to each pellet retrieval forcing renewed orientation and preventing movement execution from the same angle. Second, pellets are grasped from a vertical post that matches the diameter of the pellet and is placed in front of the cage. This requires a precise grasp for successful pellet retrieval and thus prevents simple pulling of the pellet towards the animal. In combination with novel genetics, imaging and electrophysiological technologies, this behavioral method will aid to understand the morphological, anatomical and molecular underpinnings of motor learning and memory.

Wprowadzenie

Movement control is a core function of the central nervous system (CNS). Motricity is the main measurable output of CNS function and the main possibility for individuals to interact with the external world. Understanding the principles of motor function and the mechanisms that underlie the learning of a motor task is currently one of the big challenges in neuroscience. Morphological, physiological and molecular changes were found upon acquisition of a new motor task. For example, the shape and number of synapses change in response to skilled motor training1-5, and functional changes of the synaptic machinery were observed after motor learning. Synaptic responses were higher in the connections of the forelimb-representing region of the trained motor cortex compared to the untrained hemisphere of the same animal or to responses from untrained animals6,7. Electrophysiological observations also suggest that long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) like mechanisms take place during the learning of a new motor skill, and that the range of synaptic operation, which is defined between the limiting borders of LTP and LTD saturation, is modified8. Furthermore, it has been shown that activity markers and plasticity promoting molecules such as c-fos, GAP-43, or BDNF but also plasticity inhibiting molecules such as Nogo-A display regulatory roles for learning-related neuronal plasticity9-16.

These advances towards a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying motor learning could only be achieved with the use of behavioral paradigms that allow precise control of the acquisition of a new motor skill, e.g., skilled forelimb-reaching. Only a well-structured behavioral task allows to monitor and capture correlative changes that occur upon learning and execution of the respective task. Here we visually demonstrate a modified version of the skilled forelimb single-pellet reaching task in rats adapted from Buitrago et al.17 The presented paradigm allows the analysis of movement acquisition within a daily training session (within-session) representing the fast learning component and primary acquisition as well as skilled motor learning over several sessions (between-session) representing the slow learning component and maintenance of the learned task18. Importantly, this behavioral paradigm increases the degree of difficulty and complexity of the motor skill task due to two features: First, the rats are trained to turn around their axis after each grasp and thus to realign their body prior to the next pellet reach and renew the body orientation, preventing constant movement execution from the same angle. Second, pellets are retrieved from a vertical post placed in front of the cage. Due to the small diameter of the post, pellets can easily be kicked off requiring a precise grasp for successful retrieval and preventing simple pulling of the pellet towards the animal.

Such complex behavioral testing allows deeper insights into the mechanisms underlying motor learning. Compared to mice, rats are superior in their performance of complex behavioral tasks and thus better suited for complex paradigms as presented in this study. Considering the increasing genetic possibilities available for rats19,20, the combination of precise and well controlled behavioral testing methods with genetic manipulations, imaging and physiological techniques represents a powerful toolbox to better understand the neurobiological basis of motor learning and memory.

Protokół

All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Veterinary Office of the Canton Zurich, Switzerland.

1. Animal Handling and Habituation

  1. Animal Handling
    Note: 5 days prior to the start of the experiment, perform step 1.1.1 daily.
    1. For behavioral experiments, accustom the animals to the experimenter. Have daily handling sessions lasting 10-15 min per animal. Clean the box after each animal’s session.
      1. Initially, place the experimenter’s hand into a cage allowing the animal to explore and smell to become familiarized with the experimenter.
      2. Next, gently lift the animal with the experimenter’s hand in a safe manner by grasping the rat’s body between the front- and hind paws allowing further familiarization.
    2. Weigh each animal daily to obtain a baseline bodyweight before food deprivation.
  2. Apparatus Habituation and Food Familiarization
    1. Weigh each animal daily to obtain a baseline bodyweight before food deprivation.
    2. Start food depriving rats 3 days prior to the beginning of pre-training on a standard laboratory diet. Give rats 0.05 g of food per 1 g bodyweight per day (e.g., a rat weighing 200 g, start with 10 g of food). Ensure bodyweight to not decrease more than 10% per day by monitoring bodyweight daily.
      1. If several animals are held in one cage, dominant rats may eat more than less dominant ones. In case the bodyweight of an animal decreases, feed the animal separately instead of group feeding. Give water ad libitum.
    3. To acquaint the rat with the grasping apparatus, place the animal into the training box (Figure 1). Have sugar pellets placed in the training box in proximity to the slit opening to familiarize the animal with the food pellets. Perform this step 10-15 min daily for 3 days.

2. Pre-training and Motor Skill Learning

  1. Pre-training
    1. One day after familiarization, place the animal into the training box and place the pellet closely to the slit opening so it can be reached by the animal’s tongue. Exclude animals that retrieve pellets with their forelimb during pre-training
      Note: At this stage, pellet retrieval with the tongue is crucial and usually the animal’s method of choice. Pellets should be in no case grasped with the forelimb until the first day of motor skill learning to allow adequate monitoring of the learned reaching task. Pellet retrieval with the forelimb during pre-training is an exclusion criterion and is in most cases not observed.
    2. Teach the rat to either run to the rear of the cage and return to the slit opening or step back and turn around its own axis in order to receive the next food pellet with the tongue. Allow time for the animal to explore the cage, run to the rear and return to the slit opening. If the animal does not correctly execute the task, use a forceps to gently tap at the end rear of the cage and catch the animal’s attention. Once the animal is at the rear, gently tap at the cage front to guide the animal to the slit opening.
      Note: Once the animal reaches a defined standard value (e.g., 50 successful pellet retrievals with the tongue in less than 15 min), the animal qualifies for the motor skill learning stage. On day 1 and 2 of pre-training, learners can already be distinguished from non-learners. Non-learners can be excluded from the study at this step. This decreases the probability of having a high number of non-learners during the motor learning step (2.2).
    3. During pre-training, food deprive rats on a standard laboratory diet. Give water ad libitum. Monitor body weight daily throughout the study. Do not use same room to train male and female rats. Ensure a calm and noise-free environment for the animal.
  2. Determination of Paw Preference and Motor Skill Learning
    1. During the first session of motor skill learning, replace the slide in front of the window with a post. Place the sugar pellet about 1.5 cm away from the window on the post so that the animal cannot reach the pellet with its tongue but only retrieve it by a precise forelimb reaching and grasping movement.
      1. To enforce pellet retrieval by the forelimb, use a forceps to gently bring the pellet close to the animal’s mouth and retract the pellet while the animal attempts consumption with the tongue. Carry out this task repetitively until the animal stretches out the forelimb and grasps a pellet.
    2. Place the post central to the window opening. To determine paw preference, carefully observe the first 10 trials on training day 1. More than 70% of the reaching attempts (i.e., 7 out of 10) have to be executed with the same forelimb. If this is not achieved, continue with another round of 10 trials until the 70% threshold is reached.
    3. After paw preference determination, shift the post towards the preferred forelimb and align central to the border of the window opening. Preferred paw alignment means the post is shifted contrary to the respective paw to allow an optimal angle for reaching (Figure 1B, C).
    4. Classify a trial, defined as a new pellet presented to the animal, as successful (reach, grasp, retrieve and eat the pellet), drop (reach, grasp and lose pellet during retrieval) or fail (knock pellet off the post). Note down every trial in your sheet and analyse the data after the experiment.
    5. Perform one daily session consisting of a defined number of trials (e.g., 150) or a maximum time (e.g., 1 hr) for each animal.
    6. Investigate precision and fine-tuning of the movement using first attempt analysis. A first attempt is defined by grasping of the pellet in a single monolithic movement without disruption, hesitation or repetition of individual movement components. Carefully observe each grasp by the rat.
      1. If the rat hesitates or retracts during a reach or attempts several trials to correctly grasp the pellet, note the respective trial as success but not first attempt. If the animal successfully grasps the pellet in a single monolithic reach, note down the respective trial as successful first attempt in a separate column in your sheet.
    7. During motor skill learning, food deprive rats on a standard laboratory diet. Give water ad libitum. Monitor body weight daily throughout the study.

Wyniki

Successful motor skill acquisition is only achieved through consistent practice. Despite careful consideration of all aspects, some rats fail to learn the task (Figure 2). These 'non-learners' either lack motivation resulting in few or absent attempts of pellet retrieval from the start of the experiment or generally lose interest in reaching for the pellets leading to continuously failed attempts. By contrast, some animals show aggressive and over-motivated behavior resulting in overhasty and rus...

Dyskusje

The paradigm shown in this study is adapted from Buitrago et al.18 and differs from the classical single pellet reaching paradigm17 mainly in two aspects:

First, studying within-session improvement allows analysis of the learned task within a single day, which can provide a different level of information such as investigation of the fast learning component compared to the slow learning component represented by the average daily values (see Figures 3

Ujawnienia

The authors have nothing to disclose

Podziękowania

This work was funded by grants of the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant 31003A-149315-1 to M.E.S. and Grant IZK0Z3-150809 to A.Z.), to A.Z. the Heidi Demetriades Foundation, to M.E.S. the European Research Council (‘Nogorise’) and the Christopher and Dana Reeve Foundation (CDRF).

Materiały

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
Training boxSelf Made
PedestalSelf Made
Sugar pelletsTSE Systems Intl. Group45 mg dustless precision pellets
Sprague Dawley rats5-6 week old males
Laptop computerHewlett Packard
Stop Watch
ForcepsFine Science Tools (FST)
ExcelMicrosoft
PrismGraphPad
Weighing scale
Counter

Odniesienia

  1. Fu, M., Yu, X., Lu, J., Zuo, Y. Repetitive motor learning induces coordinated formation of clustered dendritic spines in vivo. Nature. 483, 92-95 (2012).
  2. Fu, M., Zuo, Y. Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the cortex. Trends in neurosciences. 34, 177-187 (2011).
  3. Holtmaat, A., Svoboda, K. Experience-dependent structural synaptic plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 10, 647-658 (2009).
  4. Xu, T., et al. Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature. 462, 915-919 (2009).
  5. Yu, X., Zuo, Y. Spine plasticity in the motor cortex. Current opinion in neurobiology. 21, 169-174 (2011).
  6. Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., Friedman, D., Donoghue, J. P. . Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science. 290, 533-536 (2000).
  7. Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., Friedman, D., Hess, G., Donoghue, J. P. Strengthening of horizontal cortical connections following skill learning. Nature neuroscience. 1, 230-234 (1998).
  8. Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., Donoghue, J. P., Dunaevsky, A. Plasticity of the synaptic modification range. Journal of neurophysiology. 98, 3688-3695 (2007).
  9. Fritsch, B., et al. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron. 66, 198-204 (2010).
  10. Ghiani, C. A., Ying, Z., de Vellis, J., Gomez-Pinilla, F. Exercise decreases myelin-associated glycoprotein expression in the spinal cord and positively modulates neuronal growth. Glia. 55, 966-975 (2007).
  11. Josephson, A., et al. Activity-induced and developmental downregulation of the Nogo receptor. Cell and tissue research. 311, 333-342 (2003).
  12. Karlen, A., et al. Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106, 20476-20481 (2009).
  13. Kleim, J. A., Lussnig, E., Schwarz, E. R., Comery, T. A., Greenough, W. T. Synaptogenesis and Fos expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after motor skill learning. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 16, 4529-4535 (1996).
  14. Mironova, Y. A., Giger, R. J. Where no synapses go: gatekeepers of circuit remodeling and synaptic strength. Trends in neurosciences. 14, 7-23 (2013).
  15. Park, H., Poo, M. M. Neurotrophin regulation of neural circuit development and function. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 14, 7-23 (2013).
  16. Zemmar, A., et al. Neutralization of Nogo-A enhances synaptic plasticity in the rodent motor cortex and improves motor learning in vivo. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 34, 8685-8698 (2014).
  17. Whishaw, I. Q., Pellis, S. M. The structure of skilled forelimb reaching in the rat: a proximally driven movement with a single distal rotatory component. Behavioural brain research. 41, 49-59 (1990).
  18. Buitrago, M. M., Ringer, T., Schulz, J. B., Dichgans, J., Luft, A. R. Characterization of motor skill and instrumental learning time scales in a skilled reaching task in rat. Behavioural brain research. 155, 249-256 (2004).
  19. Geurts, A. M., et al. Knockout rats via embryo microinjection of zinc-finger nucleases. Science. 325, 433 (2009).
  20. Tews, B., et al. Synthetic microRNA-mediated downregulation of Nogo-A in transgenic rats reveals its role as regulator of synaptic plasticity and cognitive function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. , (2013).
  21. Li, D., et al. Heritable gene targeting in the mouse and rat using a CRISPR-Cas system. Nature. 31, 681-683 (2013).

Przedruki i uprawnienia

Zapytaj o uprawnienia na użycie tekstu lub obrazów z tego artykułu JoVE

Zapytaj o uprawnienia

Przeglądaj więcej artyków

Keywords Skilled Forelimb Reaching TaskMotor LearningMotor FunctionBehavioral ParadigmMotor Skill TaskPellet RetrievalGrasp PrecisionMotor Learning And Memory

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Prywatność

Warunki Korzystania

Zasady

Badania

Edukacja

O JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. Wszelkie prawa zastrzeżone