Sign In

A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content. Sign in or start your free trial.

In This Article

  • Summary
  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Protocol
  • Results
  • Discussion
  • Disclosures
  • Acknowledgements
  • Materials
  • References
  • Reprints and Permissions

Summary

Cognitive deficits are common in about one third of patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, a neurological condition leading to progressive impairments in speech and movement abilities. To conduct cognitive tests in patients unable to speak or write a reliable and easy to administer eye-tracking paradigm was developed.

Abstract

Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) is a neurodegenerative disorder with pathological involvement of upper and lower motoneurons, subsequently leading to progressive loss of motor and speech abilities. In addition, cognitive functions are impaired in a subset of patients. To evaluate these potential deficits in severely physically impaired ALS patients, eye-tracking is a promising means to conduct cognitive tests. The present article focuses on how eye movements, an indirect means of communication for physically disabled patients, can be utilized to allow for detailed neuropsychological assessment. The requirements, in terms of oculomotor parameters that have to be met for sufficient eye-tracking in ALS patients are presented. The properties of stimuli, including type of neuropsychological tests and style of presentation, best suited to successfully assess cognitive functioning, are also described. Furthermore, recommendations regarding procedural requirements are provided. Overall, this methodology provides a reliable, easy to administer and fast approach for assessing cognitive deficits in patients who are unable to speak or write such as patients with severe ALS. The only confounding factor might be deficits in voluntary eye movement control in a subset of ALS patients.

Introduction

Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) is a fatal neurodegenerative disorder usually leading to death within 3 to 5 years. In the course of the pathology, patients present with progressive loss of respiratory and bulbar functioning as well as impairments in movement abilities1. It shares some clinical, pathological, and genetic features with frontotemporal dementia2 and it is well documented that about 30% of ALS patients exhibit cognitive deficits3. These deficits are most prominent in the domains of executive function, verbal fluency and language4 and have an influence on survival5, compliance6 and carer burden7. Thus, reliable neuropsychological assessment is crucial in this disease.

Advancing impairments in motor and speech abilities are, however, a limiting factor for thorough evaluation of cognitive abilities in later stages of the disease8. Here, oculomotor based approaches seem to be very promising, as basic eye movement control remains intact for a comparably long time during the course of ALS for the majority of patients9. Eye-tracking parameters itself have been used to gain information about the cognitive status of patients with ALS10 and also correlate with the sequential spreading pattern of ALS11. Eye movement as a means to control cognitive tests in the context of ALS has also been studied in previous works. One study has successfully demonstrated its usability in healthy controls using an oculomotor based version of the Trail-Making Test12, whereas another found it suitable to distinguish between healthy controls and ALS patients based on cognitive performance and to discriminate between cognitively more and less impaired patients13.

The research described here used an oculomotor based methodology to study cognitive impairments in ALS patients, specifically in the domain of executive functioning. Two well validated and commonly used neuropsychological tests were adapted to eye movement control: the Raven's coloured progressive matrices (CPM)14 and the D2-test15. The CPM is a non-verbal instrument used to measure executive and visuospatial abilities as well as fluid intelligence. The D2-test is also a non-verbal tool used to uncover executive dysfunction in the domains of selective and sustained attention and visual processing speed. Both are widely used clinical tools which have been successfully employed in previous studies assessing potential cognitive decline during the course of the disease16 and the neuropsychological status of ALS patients compared to healthy controls17.

The goal of this work was to show the requirements for successful evaluation of cognitive deficits in ALS independent of movement and speech disabilities using a reliable, eye-tracking based version of the CPM and the D2-test. Importantly, the method described here has the potential to be expanded to study other populations of patients with severe motor impairments.

Protocol

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Ulm (Statement No. 19/12) and the protocol described therefore follows their guidelines. All participants gave written informed consent.

1. Stimuli and Testing Environment

  1. To keep distractions to a minimum perform the research in a dark or a very dimly lit and quiet room.
  2. Use an appropriate eye tracking device.
    NOTE: There are a wide variety of devices available to perform eye movement studies. In the present research a portable eye movement recording device with goggles was used, which synchronously measure binocular eye positions, using two integrated cameras with an infrared light emitting diode (IR LEDs), one for each eye18.
    1. To ensure optimal eye-tracking, adjust the cameras manually by tilting them in all 6 degrees of freedom (3 translational, 3 rotational) until eye movement detection is optimal.
      NOTE: The measurements of the system are displayed in real-time on an experimenter's screen to monitor recording quality and participants' response behavior. The key features of the system are described below and detailed specifications are given in Table 1.
  3. Implement a standardized oculomotor testing paradigm, which is able to detect deficits in eye movement control.
    NOTE: This can be a series of tasks like smooth pursuit or saccade tasks, in which participants with impaired eye movement control perform badly (e.g., the methods described by Gorges et al. 201511). These participants have to be excluded from further testing.
  4. Present tasks using appropriate software that projects the stimuli on a hemi-cylindrical screen via a projector mounted above the subject's head11. Additionally, ensure that a red laser spot (diameter: 0.3°, position: 10° vertical) is present for the D2-test.
  5. Seat the participants on an elevated chair with an adjustable chin rest, so that eye-to-screen distance is approximately 150 cm.
  6. Use set A and B of the CPM (12 stimuli for each set) for the first part of the experiment. Show these stimuli as 22° long/15° high matrices with six possible alternatives depicted below, which might fit in a 6° wide and 5° high blank space cut out from the matrix.
  7. For the D2-test, use the five blocks with 47 stimuli, corresponding to line 2 - 6 of the standard D2-test15, and depict them one stimulus after another in the middle of the screen with a height of 11° and a width of 2.5°. Make sure each stimulus lasts for 2,000 msec.

2. Running the Experiment

  1. Before the experiment starts, ask participants to fill out a written informed consent form to ensure the study is in accordance with ethical standards.
  2. Give general instructions concerning the purpose and procedure of the experiment and control for CNS-active drugs, i.e., ask the participant if he/she is currently taking any medication which affects alertness and therefore cognitive performance.
  3. Turn off all devices which might be a potential disturbance, such as mobile phones or pagers.
  4. Seat participants comfortably with the chin rest in optimal position. Ensure that the whole screen is visible and ask the participant to maintain posture during the whole experiment.
  5. Ask participants to place their head on the chin rest and place the videooculography goggles onto their head. Adjust them to individual head size/shape.
    NOTE: Thereby, the best possible compromise between comfort for the participant and minimal risk of unwanted slipping during measurement must be made.
  6. Make sure that both eyes are visible on the experimenter's screen, where the images of the two eye movement recording cameras within the goggles are displayed and then focus the cameras to center the images on the screen.
    NOTE: This is important for optimal detection and tracking of the pupil as an improper acuity can lead to a disturbed signal.
  7. To ensure continuous recording of eye movements at all times, start calibration of the system by instructing the participant to look in each corner of the hemi-cylindrical screen. In case the signal is lost, change the angle of the camera tracking the respective pupil of the eye to resolve the problem.
  8. Start the standardized oculomotor testing paradigm described above and exclude all patients with substantial visual impairments as these will cause corrupted task performance.
  9. Instruct the participant to track a single spot on the screen, oscillating horizontally (± 20°) and then vertically (± 15°) with a frequency of 0.125 Hz to map the non-calibrated orthogonalized 'raw' data from the eye movement recording device with respect to the 'true' orthogonalized eye position for calibration of the system.
  10. Check if calibration is acceptable, i.e., if the 'true' eye movement and the 'raw' data are spatially and temporally synchronized and then instruct the participant to not move the head as this might result in bad data quality.
    NOTE: Position of the videooculography device and the subject is expected to be stationary during the course of the experiment, so that there is no need for re-calibration.
  11. Explain the procedure of the training session for the CPM:
    1. Instruct the subjects to identify the missing pieces below the upcoming matrices displayed on the hemi-cylindrical screen for which they have infinite time. After making a choice, have the subject close their eyes for at least 250 msec to start off a green frame outlining all of the possible alternatives of missing pieces for 1,500 msec each.
    2. Have the participant choose the alternative they think is correct by closing their eyes for at least 250 msec while their choice is enframed.
    3. Project the choice separately on the hemi-cylindrical screen. Ask the participants to confirm. If the participant confirms, instruct them to close their eyes again for at least 250 msec.
    4. Instruct the participants that the next stimulus (matrix with 6 alternatives of missing pieces) will automatically (again, presented by the software described in step 2) appear and that this will be done four times with training stimuli taken from set AB of the CPM14.
  12. Answer any questions subjects might have about the procedure. Then, instruct the participants to choose the missing piece of the matrix, as they have learned during the training session and start the CPM (Figure 1).
  13. Explain the procedure of the training session for the D2-test:
    1. Instruct participants to direct their gaze to the center of the screen and observe 47 stimuli separately (47 d's corresponding to line 1 of the standard D2-test).
    2. Instruct the participants to look at the red laser spot positioned above the stimuli whenever a "d" with two dashes is presented until the next stimulus appears. NOTE: If no target stimulus is presented the gaze needs to be focused at the center of the screen where the next stimulus will be presented.
  14. Answer any questions subjects might have about the procedure. Then, instruct the participant to proceed for the following five blocks of 47 stimuli, similar to the training session and start the D2-test.
  15. For quality control, inspect the data obtained from each session for each subject carefully (visually by a trained experimenter). Check if data quality is corrupted, for example due to technical difficulties, corrupted eye movements or misunderstandings.
  16. Thank subjects for their participation and answer all questions during the experiment.

Results

For the purpose of the research presented here, i.e., the development of a reliable oculomotor based neuropsychological assessment for ALS patients, in-house developed software stores the subject's choices of the CPM in a separate file, allowing for manual computation of the percentage of correct answers. For the D2-test, a record of the vertical eye movements is manually analyzed using a threshold of + 5° for detection of a relevant response. Recordings are assigne...

Discussion

It is a challenging task to successfully assess the cognitive status of patients with ALS who are unable to speak and write. The use of videooculography systems provides a promising approach. The hereby presented technique is reliable in detecting cognitive deficits, which play a pivotal role in the context of carer burden and disease management20 in ALS patients. Also, the oculomotor versions of the CPM and the D2-test correlate significantly with their respective standard paper-pencil versions, but further s...

Disclosures

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Ralf Kühne for technical support. This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) and the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF #01GM1103A). This is an EU Joint Programme-Neurodegenerative Disease Research (JPND) project. The project is supported through the following organizations under the aegis of JPND-e.g., Germany, Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF, FKZ), Sweden, Vetenskaprådet Sverige, Poland, Narodowe Centrum Badań i Rozwoju (NCBR).

Materials

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
EyeSeeCamEyeSeeTec GmbH; 82256 Fürstenfeldbruck, GermanyVideooculography device

References

  1. Kiernan, M. C., et al. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Lancet. 377 (9769), 942-955 (2011).
  2. Neumann, M., et al. Ubiquitinated TDP-43 in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Science. 314 (5796), 130-133 (2006).
  3. Beeldman, E., Raaphorst, J., Klein Twennaar, ., de Visser, M., Schmand, B. A., de Haan, R. J. The cognitive profile of ALS: a systematic review and meta-analysis update. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. , (2015).
  4. Phukan, J., et al. The syndrome of cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a population-based study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 83 (1), 102-108 (2012).
  5. Elamin, M., et al. Executive dysfunction is a negative prognostic indicator in patients with ALS without dementia. Neurology. 76 (14), 1263-1269 (2011).
  6. Martin, N. A., et al. Psychological as well as illness factors influence acceptance of non-invasive ventilation (NIV) and gastrostomy in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS): a prospective population study. Amyotroph. Lateral. Scler. Frontotemporal. Degener. 15 (5-6), 376-387 (2014).
  7. Chiò, A., et al. Neurobehavioral symptoms in ALS are negatively related to caregivers' burden and quality of life. Eur. J. Neurol. 17 (10), 1298-1303 (2010).
  8. Lakerveld, J., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A. Cognitive function in patients with late stage amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 79 (1), 25-29 (2008).
  9. Sharma, R., Hicks, S., Berna, C. M., Kennard, C., Talbot, K., Turner, M. R. Oculomotor dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a comprehensive review. Arch. Neurol. 68 (7), 857-861 (2011).
  10. Witiuk, K., Fernandez-Ruiz, J., McKee, R. Cognitive Deterioration and Functional Compensation in ALS Measured with fMRI Using an Inhibitory Task. J. Neurosci. 34 (43), 14260-14271 (2014).
  11. Gorges, M., et al. Eye Movement Deficits Are Consistent with a Staging Model of pTDP-43 Pathology in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis. PLoS One. 10 (11), 0142546 (2015).
  12. Hicks, S. L., et al. An eye-tracking version of the trail-making test. PLoS One. 8 (12), 84061 (2013).
  13. Keller, J., et al. Eye-tracking controlled cognitive function tests in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a controlled proof-of-principle study. J. Neurol. 262 (8), 1918-1926 (2015).
  14. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., Raven, J. . Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 2, the coloured progressive matrices. , (1998).
  15. Brickenkamp, R. . Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (Test d2), 8th edn. , (1994).
  16. Elamin, M., et al. Cognitive changes predict functional decline in ALS. Neurology. 80 (17), 1590-1597 (2013).
  17. Ludolph, A. C., et al. Frontal lobe function in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a neuropsychologic and positron emission tomography study. Acta. Neurol. Scand. 85 (2), 81-89 (1992).
  18. Schneider, E., et al. Eye-SeeCam: an eye movement-driven head camera for the examination of natural visual exploration. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1164, 461-467 (2009).
  19. Lulé, D., et al. The Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioural Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Screen: a cross-sectional comparison of established screening tools in a German-Swiss population. Amyotroph. Lateral. Scler. Frontotemporal. Degener. 16 (1-2), 16-23 (2015).
  20. Olney, R. K., et al. The effects of executive and behavioral dysfunction in the course of ALS. Neurology. 65 (11), 1774-1777 (2005).
  21. Donaghy, C., Thurtell, M. J., Pioro, E. P., Gibson, J. M., Leigh, R. J. Eye movements in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and its mimics: a review with illustrative cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 82 (1), 110-116 (2011).
  22. Mizutani, T., et al. Development of ophthalmoplegia in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis during long-term use of respirators. J. Neurol. Sci. 99 (2-3), 311-319 (1990).
  23. Khin Khin, E., Minor, D., Holloway, A., Pelleg, A. Decisional Capacity in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis. J. Am. Acad. Psychiatry Law. 43 (2), 210-217 (2015).
  24. Kübler, A., et al. Patients with ALS can use sensorimotor rhythms to operate a brain-computer interface. Neurology. 64 (10), 1775-1777 (2005).
  25. Connolly, S., Galvin, M., Hardiman, O. End-of-life management in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 14 (4), 435-442 (2015).

Reprints and Permissions

Request permission to reuse the text or figures of this JoVE article

Request Permission

Explore More Articles

Keywords Eye trackingCognitive FunctionsAmyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis ALSOculomotor ControlSaccadic TasksD2 TestVideo oculographyEye Movement Recording

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Privacy

Terms of Use

Policies

Research

Education

ABOUT JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved