Sign In

Children's Reliance on Artist Intentions When Identifying Pictures

Overview

Source: Laboratories of Judith Danovitch and Nicholaus Noles—University of Louisville

Children are not the best artists. Sometimes it’s easy to pick out the characteristic triangular head, whiskers, and tail of a cat, but children often describe elaborate scenarios that they depict as a beautifully unrecognizable mess. Thus, given children’s questionable artistic talent, how do they know what their drawings, and the drawings of others, represent? One way children identify pictures is by relying on resemblance. If it looks like a cat, then it’s a cat. However, some pictures do not clearly resemble any real object. In this situation, children must use other means to figure out what the picture represents, including their understanding of what the person who created the picture intended it to represent.

By their first birthday, children are sensitive to the intentions of other people. They know that people’s actions are driven by their goals, and they can infer a person’s intentions even if the goal-directed action is not successful (e.g., they understand a person struggling to turn a lid intends to open a jar, even if they never see them succeed in opening it). By about age 3, children can use this understanding of intention to guide their interpretation of drawings and other pictorial representations. They apply this understanding both to identifying their own drawings, and to interpreting drawings created by another person.

This experiment demonstrates how to measure children’s use of intention to interpret otherwise ambiguous pictures based on the methods developed by Bloom and Markson.1

Procedure

Recruit 3- and 4-year-old children. Participants should have no history of developmental disorders and have normal hearing and vision. For the purposes of this demonstration, only one child is tested. Larger sample sizes (as in the Bloom and Markson study1) are recommended when conducting any experiments.

1. Data collection

  1. Drawing task: Part 1
    1. Prepare a crayon and four sheets of blank paper. Invite the child into the room and have them sit at a tab

Log in or to access full content. Learn more about your institution’s access to JoVE content here

Results

Researchers tested 24 4-year-olds and found that 4-year-old children correctly identified 87% of the pictures in the drawing task (Figure 1). They also identified 83% of the pictures correctly in the size task and 68% of the pictures in the oddity task. All of these results were significantly better than chance at the p < 0.05 level. The researchers also tested 24 3-year-olds and found that they identified 76% of the pictures in the drawing task and 69% of the pictures in the size task correct

Log in or to access full content. Learn more about your institution’s access to JoVE content here

Application and Summary

Pictures and drawings are symbols, and the ability to identify what a symbol represents is important for the development of a wide range of skills. As early as age 3, children realize that understanding the intentions of a drawing’s creator can allow them to identify a drawing that might otherwise be unidentifiable. Moreover, children as young as age 3 can do this even if the drawing bears no resemblance to the intended object. Although appearance and shape are certainly still valuable for identifying pictorial re

Log in or to access full content. Learn more about your institution’s access to JoVE content here

References
  1. Bloom, P., & Markson, L. Intention and analogy in children's naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science. 9(3), 200-204 (1998).
Tags
Children s RelianceArtist IntentionsIdentifying PicturesInterpretationPhysical CharacteristicsSocial cognitive ProcessesResemblanceAbstract PicturesArtist s IntentionExperimental DesignData CollectionSymbolsAbstract ArtPhysical Objects3 4 Year Old ChildrenTasksHand drawings

Skip to...

0:00

Overview

1:15

Experimental Design

4:14

Running the Experiment

6:37

Representative Results

7:39

Applications

9:13

Summary

Videos from this collection:

article

Now Playing

Children's Reliance on Artist Intentions When Identifying Pictures

Developmental Psychology

5.6K Views

article

Habituation: Studying Infants Before They Can Talk

Developmental Psychology

53.1K Views

article

Using Your Head: Measuring Infants' Rational Imitation of Actions

Developmental Psychology

10.1K Views

article

The Rouge Test: Searching for a Sense of Self

Developmental Psychology

53.8K Views

article

Numerical Cognition: More or Less

Developmental Psychology

14.9K Views

article

Mutual Exclusivity: How Children Learn the Meanings of Words

Developmental Psychology

32.6K Views

article

How Children Solve Problems Using Causal Reasoning

Developmental Psychology

13.0K Views

article

Metacognitive Development: How Children Estimate Their Memory

Developmental Psychology

10.4K Views

article

Executive Function and the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task

Developmental Psychology

14.8K Views

article

Categories and Inductive Inferences

Developmental Psychology

5.2K Views

article

The Costs and Benefits of Natural Pedagogy

Developmental Psychology

5.1K Views

article

Piaget's Conservation Task and the Influence of Task Demands

Developmental Psychology

59.6K Views

article

Measuring Children's Trust in Testimony

Developmental Psychology

6.3K Views

article

Are You Smart or Hardworking? How Praise Influences Children's Motivation

Developmental Psychology

14.2K Views

article

Memory Development: Demonstrating How Repeated Questioning Leads to False Memories

Developmental Psychology

10.7K Views

JoVE Logo

Privacy

Terms of Use

Policies

Research

Education

ABOUT JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved