Aby wyświetlić tę treść, wymagana jest subskrypcja JoVE. Zaloguj się lub rozpocznij bezpłatny okres próbny.
This study presents a naturalistic experimental setup that allows researchers to present real-time action stimuli, obtain response time and mouse tracking data while participants respond after each stimulus display, and change actors between experimental conditions with a unique system including a special transparent organic light-emitting diode (OLED) screen and light manipulation.
Perception of others' actions is crucial for survival, interaction, and communication. Despite decades of cognitive neuroscience research dedicated to understanding the perception of actions, we are still far away from developing a neurally inspired computer vision system that approaches human action perception. A major challenge is that actions in the real world consist of temporally unfolding events in space that happen "here and now" and are actable. In contrast, visual perception and cognitive neuroscience research to date have largely studied action perception through 2D displays (e.g., images or videos) that lack the presence of actors in space and time, hence these displays are limited in affording actability. Despite the growing body of knowledge in the field, these challenges must be overcome for a better understanding of the fundamental mechanisms of the perception of others' actions in the real world. The aim of this study is to introduce a novel setup to conduct naturalistic laboratory experiments with live actors in scenarios that approximate real-world settings. The core element of the setup used in this study is a transparent organic light-emitting diode (OLED) screen through which participants can watch the live actions of a physically present actor while the timing of their presentation is precisely controlled. In this work, this setup was tested in a behavioral experiment. We believe that the setup will help researchers reveal fundamental and previously inaccessible cognitive and neural mechanisms of action perception and will be a foundation for future studies investigating social perception and cognition in naturalistic settings.
A fundamental skill for survival and social interaction is the ability to perceive and make sense of others' actions and interact with them in the surrounding environment. Previous research in the last several decades has made significant contributions to understanding the fundamental principles of how individuals perceive and understand others' actions1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11. Nevertheless, given the complexity of interactions and the circumstances in which they occur, there is an obvious need to further develop the body of knowledge in naturalistic settings in order to reach a more complete understanding of this complex skill in daily life settings.
In natural environments such as our daily life settings, perception and cognition exhibit embodied, embedded, extended, and enactive characteristics12. In contrast to internalist accounts of brain functions that tend to understate the roles of the body and the environment, contemporary approaches to embodied cognition focus on the dynamic coupling of the brain, body, and environment. On the other hand, most social psychology, cognitive psychology, and neuroscience research on action perception tend to assume that utilizing well-controlled and simplified experiment designs in laboratory conditions (e.g., images or videos in computerized tasks) yields results that can be generalized to more complex scenarios such as real-world interactions1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11. This assumption guarantees that robust and reliable data can be obtained under many circumstances. Nevertheless, a well-known challenge is that the validity of the models derived from carefully controlled experiments is limited when tested in a real-world context13. Consequently, further investigations13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22 have been conducted to address the ecological and external validity of stimuli and experimental designs in various fields of research.
In this study, a novel method is suggested for investigating how individuals perceive and evaluate others' actions by using live actions performed by a real, physically present actor. Scenarios similar to real-life contexts are employed, while the experimenters have control over possible confounding factors. This study is a form of "naturalistic laboratory research", within the framework of Matusz et al.14 which can be conceived as an intermediate stage between "classic laboratory research", which makes use of maximal control over the stimuli and environment, often at the expense of naturalness, and "fully naturalistic real-world research", which aims to maximize naturalness at the expense of control over the stimulation and the environment14. The study aims to address the need for empirical investigations at this level in action perception research in order to bridge the gap between the findings obtained in traditional laboratory experiments with a high degree of experimental control and the findings obtained in studies conducted in entirely unconstrained, natural settings.
Controlled versus unconstrained experiments
Experimental control is an efficient strategy for designing experiments to test a specific hypothesis, as it allows researchers to isolate target variables from likely confounding factors. It also allows for revisiting the same hypothesis with certain levels of amendments, such as using slightly or totally different stimuli in the same design or testing the same stimuli in alternative experimental setups. Systematic investigation through controlled experiments is a traditional form of methodology in research in cognitive science and relevant domains. Controlled experiments still help to establish the body of knowledge on the fundamental principles of cognitive processes in various domains of research, such as attention, memory, and perception. However, recent research has also acknowledged the limitations of traditional laboratory experiments in terms of generalizing the findings to real-world settings, and researchers have been encouraged to conduct studies in enhanced ecological settings13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21. This shift aims to address two important issues regarding the discrepancy between traditional laboratory experiments and real-world settings. First, the world outside the laboratory is less deterministic than in experiments, which limits the representative power of systematic experimental manipulations. Second, the human brain is highly adaptive, and this is often underestimated due to the practical limitations of designing and conducting experimental studies22. The concept of "ecological validity"23,24 has been used to address methods for resolving this issue. The term is usually used to refer to a prerequisite for the generalization of experimental findings to the real world outside the laboratory context. Ecological validity has also been interpreted as referring to validating virtually naturalistic experimental setups with unconstrained stimuli to ensure that the study design is analogous to real-life scenarios25. Due to the high degree of variance in the interpretation of this term, an understanding of the advantages and limitations of alternative methodologies and stimulus selection is required.
Levels of naturalism in stimuli and experiment design
Previous work in experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience has used a wide range of stimuli with different levels of naturalism26. Most researchers prefer to use static images or short dynamic videos because these stimuli are easier to prepare than those that could simulate a real action or an event. Despite having advantages, these stimuli do not allow researchers to measure contingent behaviors among social agents. In other words, they are not actable and do not have social affordance27. In recent years, an alternative to these non-interactive stimuli has been developed: real-time animations of virtual avatars. These avatars allow for the investigation of the interactions between avatars and their users. However, the use of virtual avatars is subject to reduced user apprehension, especially when they do not appear particularly engaging in terms of their realistic and contingent behaviors26. Therefore, there is now more interest in using real social stimuli in experimental studies. Although their design, data recording, and analysis may require advanced equipment and complex data analysis, they are the best candidates for understanding naturalistic human behavior and cognition.
The present study proposes a methodology for using real-life social stimuli in a laboratory environment. This study aims to investigate how people perceive and evaluate others' actions in a setting with enhanced ecological validity compared to traditional laboratory experiments. We have developed and described a novel setup in which participants are exposed to real actors who are physically present and share the same environment with them. In this protocol, the participants' response times and mouse trajectories are measured, which requires precise timing of the stimuli presentation and strict control over the experimental conditions in this enhanced ecological setting. Therefore, the experimental paradigm stands out among the frameworks present in the literature since the naturalness of the stimuli is maximized without sacrificing control over the environment. Below, the protocol presents the steps to establish such a system and then continues with the representative results for the sample data. Finally, a discussion of the paradigm's significance, limitations, and plans for modifications is presented.
Experimental design
Before proceeding to the protocol section, we describe the parameters used in the present study and present the details of the stimuli together with the experimental design.
Parameters in the study
This study aims to measure how the type of actor and the class of actions they perform affect the mind perception processes of the participants. In the protocol, the mind perception process is measured in two main dimensions, namely agency and experience, as proposed by previous research28. The high and low ends of these two dimensions are also included, as recently introduced by Li et al.29.
The structure of the study was inspired by the single-category version30 of the commonly used implicit association task (IAT)31. In this task, the response times of the participants while they match an attribute concept with the target concept are used as an indication of the strength of their implicit associations for these two concepts. In the adaptation of this implicit task, the participants are presented live actions performed by real actors and required to match them to target concepts. The target concepts are the high and low ends of the agency or experience dimensions, depending on the block of the experiment.
To summarize, the independent variables are Actor Type and Action Class. Actor Type has two levels (i.e., two different actors, Actor1 and Actor2, performing in the study). Action Class has two levels: Action Class1 and Action Class2, and each class contains four actions. The participants evaluate the two actors separately in four blocks (one actor in each block), and in each block, the actors perform all of the actions in a counter-balanced order. The participants perform evaluations with respect to two pre-defined and forced dimensions: Agency and Experience. The four blocks in the experiment are (1) Actor1 in Agency Block, (2) Actor2 in Agency Block, (3) Actor1 in Experience Block, and (4) Actor2 in Experience Block. The order of the blocks is also counter-balanced among the participants so that the blocks with the same agent never follow each other.
Besides the answers of the participants, the response times and the x-y coordinates of the wireless mouse they use while they move toward one of the two response alternatives are recorded. So, the dependent variables are the response and the response time (RT) of the participants, as well as the measurements of maximum deviation (MD) and area under the curve (AUC), derived from the computer mouse-tracking. The variable response is categorical; it can be High or Low, and since the evaluations are done in one of the given blocks, the responses can also be labeled as High-Agency, Low-Agency, High-Experience, or Low-Experience. Response time is a continuous variable; its unit is seconds, and it refers to the elapsed time between the start of the presentation of an action and the occurrence of a mouse click on one of the response alternatives. The MD of a trajectory is a continuous variable, and it refers to the largest perpendicular deviation between the trajectory of the participant(s) and the idealized trajectory (straight line). The AUC of a trajectory is also a continuous variable, and it refers to the geometric area between the trajectory of the participant(s) and the idealized trajectory32.
Stimuli and design of the experiment
A three-staged experiment is used in the present study. The measurements from the third part are used for the analyses; the first two parts serve as preparation for the final part. Below, we describe each part of the experiment together with the experimental stimuli and hypotheses.
In Experiment Part 1 (lexical training part), the participants complete a training session to understand the concepts of Agency and Experience and the capacity levels represented with the words High and Low. To select the concepts (n = 12) to be used in this training session, some of the authors of the current work conducted a normative study33. Since the present study was planned to be conducted in the native languages of the participants, the concepts were also translated into Turkish before being normalized. Concepts were selected from among those that were strongly associated with the High (n= 3) and Low (n= 3) ends of the two dimensions (six concepts for each). This part is crucial since the participants' understanding of the concepts is expected to guide their evaluation processes.
In Experiment Part 2 (action identification part), participants watch the same eight actions performed by Actor1 and Actor2 one after the other and report what the action is to the experimenter. This section serves as a manipulation check; by presenting all the actions when both actors are performing them, it is possible to make sure that the participants understand the actions and are familiar with the actors before they start the implicit test, where they need to make fast evaluations. The actions selected for Action Class1 and Action Class2 are those that had the highest H scores and confidence levels (four different action exemplars in each action class) according to the results of the two normative studies (N = 219) for each actor condition conducted by some of the authors (manuscript in preparation). All actions are performed within an equal time duration of 6 s.
This is an ongoing study, and it has some other components; however, the hypotheses for the sections described above are as follows: (i) the type of actor will affect the dependent variables; Actor2 will yield longer RTs, higher MDs, and larger AUCs compared to Actor1; (ii) the type of action will affect the dependent measurements; Action Class1 will yield longer RTs, higher MDs, and larger AUCs compared to Action Class2; (iii) the dependent measurements for High and Low responses for the same actor and action class will differ across the block dimensions: Agency and Experience.
The experimental protocols in this study were approved by the Ethics Committee for Research with Human Participants of Bilkent University. All participants included in the study were over 18 years old, and they read and signed the informed consent form before starting the study.
1. General design steps
NOTE: Figure 1A (top view) and Figure 1B and Figure 1C (front and back views) demonstrate the laboratory layout; these figures were created with respect to the original laboratory setup and configuration designed for this particular study. Figure 1A shows the top-view layout of the lab. In this figure, it is possible to see LED lights on the ceiling and the actor cabinet. The blackout curtain system divides the room in half and helps light manipulation by preventing light from leaking into the front part of the room (Participant Area). Figure 1B presents the view of the laboratory from the perspective of the experimenter. The participant sits right in front of the OLED screen, and using the see-through display, they can watch the live actions performed by the actors. They give their responses by using the response device (a wireless mouse) in front of them. The experimenter can simultaneously watch the actor through the participant display (OLED screen) and the footage coming from the security camera. Figure 1C demonstrates the backstage of the study (Actor Area) with the security camera and the Actor personal computer (PC), which are not visible to the participant. The security camera footage goes to the Camera PC to establish communication between the actors and the experimenter. The Actor PC displays the block order and the next action information to the actor so that the experiment flows without any interruption. The actors can check the next action quickly while the participants respond to the action in the previous trial.
Figure 1: Naturalistic laboratory setup. (A) Top-down view of the naturalistic laboratory setup. (B) The back and front sides of the naturalistic experimental setup from the participant's viewpoint. (C) The back and front sides of the naturalistic experimental setup from the actor's viewpoint. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 2: System and wiring diagram. (A) The system diagram of the naturalistic experimental setup. (B) The wiring diagram of the light circuit that supports the OLED screen during the experiment. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3: OLED screen from the experimenter's viewpoint. (A) Opaque use of the OLED digital screen from the experimenter's viewpoint. (B) Transparent use of the OLED digital screen from the experimenter's viewpoint. (C) Opaque use of the OLED digital screen from the experimenter's viewpoint during a response period. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4: Backstage of the experiment. (A) Backstage during an experiment trial. (B) The actor cabinet is at the back of the OLED screen, in which the actors can wait for their turn to be visible during the experiment. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
2. Design and implementation of the lighting circuit
3. Programming of the experiment
NOTE: Create three main experimental scripts (ExperimentScript1.m [Supplemental Coding File 1], ExperimentScript2.m [Supplemental Coding File 2], and ExperimentScript3.m [Supplemental Coding File 3]), as well as several functions (RecordMouse.m [Supplemental Coding File 4], InsideROI.m [Supplemental Coding File 5], RandomizeTrials.m [Supplemental Coding File 6], RandomizeBlocks.m [Supplemental Coding File 7], GenerateResponsePage.m [Supplemental Coding File 8], GenerateTextures.m [Supplemental Coding File 9], ActorMachine.m [Supplemental Coding File 10], MatchIDtoClass.m [Supplemental Coding File 11], and RandomizeWordOrder.m [Supplemental Coding File 12]) to perform the experiment.
NOTE: Please refer to the related scripts for detailed explanations.
4. The flow of a sample experiment
Figure 5 shows a sample trial from the participant's view. Figure 5A shows the participant looking at the cursor at the center of the screen in its opaque usage. Figure 5B shows the participant watching the live-action stimuli through the screen. Figure 5C shows the evaluation screen presented to the participant after the stimuli, in which they need to drag the mouse to one of the two alternatives at each top corner of the screen.
Figure 5: OLED screen from the participant's viewpoint. (A) Opaque use of the OLED digital screen from the participant's viewpoint during a fixation screen. (B) Transparent use of the OLED digital screen from the participant's viewpoint during the presentation of a live action. (C) Opaque use of the OLED digital screen from the participant's viewpoint during the response period. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
5. Data pre-processing and analysis
6. Conditions that may lead to system failure and precautions
NOTE: In the event of system failure, it is crucial to have a physical sign (ringing a bell) to let the actor know about the failure and warn them to stay in a place that is invisible to the participant.
Response time (RT) comparisons
The current study is an ongoing project, so, as representative results, data from the main part of the experiment (Experiment Part 3) are presented. These data are from 40 participants, including 23 females and 17 males, with ages ranging from 18-28 years (M = 22.75, SD = 3.12).
Investigating the extent of the normality of the distribution of the dependent variables was necessary in order to choose the appropriate statisti...
The overarching goal of the present study is to contribute to our understanding of how human high-level visual perception and cognition work in real-life situations. This study focused on action perception and suggested a naturalistic yet controllable experimental paradigm that enables researchers to test how individuals perceive and evaluate others' actions by presenting real actors in a laboratory setting.
The significance of this proposed methodology compared to existing methodologies i...
The authors declare that they have no relevant or material financial interests related to the research described in this article.
This work was supported by grants to Burcu A. Urgen from The Scientific and Technological Research Council of Türkiye (Project number: 120K913) and Bilkent University. We thank our pilot participant Sena Er Elmas for bringing the idea of adding background noise between the actor changes, Süleyman Akı for setting up the light circuit, and Tuvana Karaduman for the idea of using a security camera backstage and her contribution as one of the actors in the study.
Name | Company | Catalog Number | Comments |
Adjustable Height Table | Custom-made | N/A | Width: 60 cm, Height: 62 cm, Depth: 40 cm |
Ardunio UNO | Smart Projects | A000066 | Microcontroller used for switching the state of the LEDs from the script running on the operator PC |
Black Pants | No brand | N/A | Relaxed-fit pants of actors with no apparent brand name or logo. |
Case | Xigmatek | EN43224 | XIGMATEK HELIOS RAINBOW LED USB 3.0 MidT ATX GAMING CASE |
CPU | AMD | YD1600BBAFBOX | AMD Ryzen 5 1600 Soket AM4 3.2 GHz - 3.6 GHz 16 MB 65 W 12 nm Processor |
Curtains | Custom-made | N/A | Width: Part 1: 110 cm width from the wall (left) side, Part 2: 123 cm width above OLED display, Part 3: 170 cm from OLED display to right side, Cabin depth: 100 cm, Inside cabin depth: 100 cm, all heights 230 cm except for Part 2 (75 cm height) |
Experimenter Adjustable/Swivel Chair | No brand | N/A | Any brand |
Experimenter Table | Custom | N/A | Width: 160 cm, Height: 75 cm, Depth: 80 cm |
GPU | MSI | GT 1030 2GHD4 LP OC | MSI GEFORCE GT 1030 2GHD4 LP OC 2GB DDR4 64bit NVIDIA GPU |
Grey-color blackout curtain | Custom-made | N/A | Width: 330 cm, Height: 230 cm, used for covering the background |
Hard Disk | Kioxia | LTC10Z240GG8 | Kioxia 240 GB Exceria Sata 3.0 SSD (555 MB Read/540 MB Write) |
Hard Disk | Toshiba | HDWK105UZSVA | Toshiba 2,5'' 500 GB L200 SATA 3.0 8 MB Cache 5400 Rpm 7 mm Harddisk |
High-Power MOSFET Module | N/A | N/A | Heating Controller MKS MOSFET Module |
Laptop | Apple | S/N: C02P916ZG3QT | MacBook Pro 11.1 Intel Core i7 (Used as the actor PC) |
Laptop | Asus | UX410U | Used for monitoring the security camera in real-time. |
LED lights | No brand | N/A | |
LED Strip Power Supply | No brand | N/A | AC to DC voltage converter used for supplying DC voltage to the lighting circuit |
MATLAB | The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA | Version: R2022a | Used for programming the experiment. Required Toolboxes: MATLAB Support Package for Arduino Hardware (version 22.1.2) Instrument Control Toolbox (version 4.6) Psychtoolbox (version 3) |
Monitor | Philips | UHB2051005145 | Model ID: 242V8A/00, PHILIPS 23.8" 242V8A 4ms 75 Hz Freesync DP-HDMI+VGA IPS Gaming Monitor |
Motherboard | MSI | B450M-A PRO MAX | MSI B450M-A PRO MAX Amd B450 Socket AM4 DDR4 3466(OC) M.2 Motherboard |
Mouse Pad for participant | Monster | 78185721101502042 / 8699266781857 | Pusat Gaming Mouse Pad XL |
Night lamp | Aukes | ES620-0.5W 6500K-IP 20 | Used for helping the actors see around when the lights are off in the backstage. |
Participant Adjustable/Swivel Chair | No brand | N/A | |
Participant Table | IKEA | Sandsberg 294.203.93 | Width: 110 cm, Height: 75 cm, Depth: 67 cm |
Power Extension Cable | Viko | 9011760Y | 250 V (6 inlets) Black |
Power Extension Cable | Viko | 9011730Y | 250 V (3 inlets) Black |
Power Extension Cable | Viko | 9011330Y | 250 V (3 inlets) White |
Power Extension Cable | s-link | Model No: SPG3-J-10 | AC - 250 V 3 meter (5 inlets) |
Power Supply | THERMALTAKE | PS-LTP-0550NHSANE-1 | THERMALTAKE LITEPOWER RGB 550W APFC 12 cm FAN PSU |
Professional Gaming Mouse | Rampage | 8680096 | Model No: SMX-R50 |
RAM | GSKILL | F4-3000C16S-8GVRB | GSKILL 8GB (1x8GB) RipjawsV Red DDR4 3000 MHz CL16 1.35 V Single Ram |
Reception bell | No brand | N/A | Used for helping the communication between the experimenter and the actors. |
Security Camera | Brion Vega | 2-20204210 | Model:BV6000 |
Speakers | Logitech | P/N: 880-000-405 PID: WD528XM | Used for playing the background music. |
Survey Software | Qualtrics | N/A | |
Switching Module | No brand | N/A | F5305S PMOS Switch Module |
Table under the OLED display | Custom-made | N/A | Width: 123 cm, Height: 75 cm, Depth: 50 cm |
Transparent OLED Display | Planar | PN: 998-1483-01 S/N:195210075 | A 55-inch transparent display that showcases dynamic information, enabled the opaque and transparent usage during the experiment. |
UPS | EAG | K200610100087 | EAG 110 |
UPS | EAG | 210312030507 | EAG 103 |
USB 2.0 Cable Type A/B for Arduino UNO (Blue) | Smart Projects | M000006 | Used to connect the microcontroller to the experimenter PC. |
USB to RS232 Converter | s-link | 8680096082559 | Model: SW-U610 |
White Long-Sleeved Blouse (2) | H&M (cotton) | N/A | Relaxed-fit blouses with a round neckline and without ant apparent brand name or logo. |
Wireless Keyboard | Logitech | P/N: 820-003488 S/N: 1719CE0856D8 | Model: K360 |
Wireless Mouse | Logitech | S/N: 2147LZ96BGQ9 | Model: M190 (Used as the response device) |
Zapytaj o uprawnienia na użycie tekstu lub obrazów z tego artykułu JoVE
Zapytaj o uprawnieniaThis article has been published
Video Coming Soon
Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. Wszelkie prawa zastrzeżone