Sign In

A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content. Sign in or start your free trial.

In This Article

  • Summary
  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Protocol
  • Results
  • Discussion
  • Disclosures
  • Acknowledgements
  • Materials
  • References
  • Reprints and Permissions

Summary

A procedure that demonstrates a robust acute food deprivation-induced relapse to heroin seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence is described. A punishment-imposed abstinence model was successfully implemented using the seek and take chain schedule for heroin self-administration. Heroin-seeking tests are then performed following 24 h of food-deprivation stress.

Abstract

The punishment-imposed abstinence procedure models the self-imposed abstinence that humans initiate due to the adverse consequences associated with drug-taking. This model has been implemented in experiments using different types of substances of abuse such as methamphetamine, cocaine, and alcohol. However, punishment-induced abstinence in heroin-trained animals has not been demonstrated. Furthermore, acute stress is a key trigger for relapse in humans and animal models. It was previously demonstrated that acute food deprivation robustly induced reinstatement of extinguished cocaine and heroin seeking. The procedure described here can be used to assess the effects of acute stress exposure on heroin seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence. A total of 8 rats were implanted with chronic intravenous (i.v.) catheters and trained to self-administer heroin (0.1 mg/kg/infusion) for 18 days under a seek-take chained schedule. Completing the seek link gave access to the take lever, which was paired with a heroin infusion. The seek lever was programmed with a variable interval 60 schedule of reinforcement (VI60), and the take lever was programmed with a fixed-ratio 1 reinforcement schedule (FR1). Following self-administration training, a mild foot shock was delivered on 30% of the completed seek links instead of the extension of the take lever. Footshock intensity was increased by 0.1 mA per daily session from 0.2 mA to 1.0 mA. Heroin-seeking tests were performed after 24 h of food deprivation (FD) or sated conditions. Rats under acute food deprivation condition robustly increased heroin seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence.

Introduction

Relapse is the most challenging problem in the treatment of drug use1,2. However, only a handful of pharmacological treatments are approved to help avoid relapse in humans3. The opioid epidemic that North America is currently facing is a striking example of it, and it demands considering different approaches on animal models of relapse to opioids.

Acute stress has been shown to be a key trigger to relapse in humans4. One environmental stressor that is often associated with drug addiction is food deprivation. Drug users often choose to allocate resources towards obtaining drugs instead of food. Caloric deficit has been shown to be correlated with higher relapse to cigarettes5 and alcohol6 use. Due to ethical and practical issues, animal models have been developed over the last decades to facilitate research in the field. In animal models, acute food deprivation has been demonstrated to robustly reinstate extinguished heroin seeking7. Currently, most animal models of relapse are based on abstinence procedures that are either not representative of human abstinence (e.g., extinction-based models) or encompass only the small percentage of drug users that are forced to abstain due to incarceration or inpatient treatment (e.g., forced abstinence models). The main reason drug users choose to abstain is the negative consequences associated with drug-seeking and taking8. Punishment-imposed abstinence is an animal model that mimics the negative consequences associated with drug-seeking on self-imposed abstinence in humans. This model introduces an aversive stimulus, e.g., a mild footshock, with drug-seeking or taking, which leads the animal to stop taking the drug voluntarily. Another procedure that incorporates negative consequences for drug-seeking is the electrical barrier conflict model for drug abstinence and relapse9. The rat must cross an electrical barrier to perform the operant behavior linked with drug self-administration. The model was used successfully to demonstrate voluntary abstinence and relapse to psychostimulant and opioid drugs10,11. However, under the electrical barrier procedure, drug-seeking efforts are always associated with an aversive event, unlike the human condition. Moreover, drug-taking itself might overlap with the electrical footshock as the animal returns to the safe area following the infusion by crossing the barrier again.

Punishment-imposed abstinence has been used with other drugs of abuse such as cocaine12, alcohol13, methamphetamine14, remifentanil15, but it was never applied to heroin-trained animals. The model has been used to study relapse induced by priming14 and drug-associated cues16, but it was not integrated into a stress-induced relapse procedure. The procedure described here is used to demonstrate acute food deprivation-induced relapse to heroin seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence in male rats.

Protocol

All rats are treated according to the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care. Approval for all the experimental procedures was granted by the Concordia University Animal Research Ethics Committee.

1. Animals

  1. Acquire Long Evans rats weighing 275-300 g (males) or 225-250 g (females). Double house rats in standard plastic cages with corncob bedding and shredded paper until surgery.
  2. Keep the rats on a reversed light-dark cycle (lights off at 9:30 AM, light on at 9:30 PM).
  3. Allow unlimited access to food and water, except during the food-deprivation test. Use regular rat chow (20.9% protein, 67.2% carbohydrates, and 11.8% fat).

2. Intravenous surgeries

  1. Build catheters and perform intravenous surgery as described in Sedki et al. 201317 with the following differences: 1) Use Isofluorane 2-2.5% for anesthesia, 2) Change the dose of penicillin administered during surgery to 60 000 IU/rat, via subcutaneous injection, and 3) Increase the dose of gentamicin in the mixture of heparin and gentamicin used to flush the catheters to 7.5 IU + 800 µg; 0.2- 0.3 mL.
  2. Inject ketoprofen or carprofen, an anti-inflammatory analgesic, intraperitoneally or subcutaneously (5 mg/kg) immediately after surgery (before recovery from the anesthesia) and on the following 3 days. During the 2 days recovery period, give mashed food and heat (if necessary) to help with the recovery. Weigh and supervise the animals daily to check for any abrupt loss of weight or any other signs of health issues.

3. Behavioral procedure

  1. Operant conditioning chambers:
    1. Set the operant conditioning chambers as described in Sedki et al. 201317.
  2. Habituation day:
    1. House the rats in the operant training chambers after recovery from surgery and allow animals to habituate to the chamber for 24 h before initiating any protocol. Keep animals housed in the operant chambers throughout the whole experiment.
    2. Do not attach the rats to the metal spring and do not initiate any experimental protocol (the cue-light, the tone, and the levers are not available during habituation day).
  3. Heroin self-administration:
    1. Attach the 5-up connector to the Tygon tube and the metal spring.
    2. Set up daily training sessions of 6 h of heroin self-administration (0.1 mg/kg/infusion) according to the procedure described below.
    3. Start daily training sessions on the onset of the dark phase of the reversed light cycle (around 9:30 AM in the representative results) with the extension of the seek lever (or take lever on the first 2 days; see below), as well as turning off the houselight on.
    4. Self-administration with only the take lever available under fixed-ratio 1 (FR1; 2 days)
      1. Program each trial to begin with the insertion of the take lever.
      2. Once the rat presses the take lever one time (FR1), retract the take lever, turn on the cue light above the lever and tone for a 20 s timeout period, turn off the houselight, and allow the delivery of a heroin infusion (0.1 mg/kg in 0.13 mL)
      3. Program a 30 s inter-trial interval in which no cues are presented, the houselight is turned off, and the lever is retracted. For the beginning of the next trial, insert the take lever, and turn on the houselight.
    5. Self-administration with seek-take chain under FR1 (5 days)
      1. Program the operant training system to insert only the seek lever and turn on the houselight to signal the beginning of the session. The take lever is in a retracted state.
      2. Once the rat presses one time (FR1) on the seek lever, the program retracts the seek lever and inserts the take lever.
      3. One press on the take lever (FR1) retracts the take lever and initiates the same procedure as in step 3.3.4.2 and a 30 s inter-trial interval.
      4. Program the operant training system to insert the seek lever at the conclusion of the inter-trial interval and turn on the houselight to signal the beginning of the next trial.
      5. Program the operant training system to initiate the inter-trial interval if a rat fails to accomplish the seek-take chain during 10 min (not pressing the seek or the take lever) to represent the end of a trial and the loss of the opportunity to administer the drug.
    6. Self-administration with seek-take chain under Variable Intervals (VI5, VI30, and VI60)
      1. Program all steps in section 3.3.5 but introduce a variable interval of 5 (VI5) as the new schedule of reinforcement on the seek lever, instead of FR1. Allow self-administration training under the VI5 schedule for 3 days.
      2. Set up the software to allow the first press on the seek lever to activate the VI5 schedule. For the VI5 schedule, let the software to randomly select an interval time from a list of 0.1 s, 5 s, and 10 s, which results in an average of 5 s interval. The first seek lever press after the selected interval has elapsed, retracts the seek lever and extends the take lever.
      3. Follow steps 3.3.4.2, 3.3.5.4., 3.3.5.5. for take lever and end of trial instructions.
      4. Next, train rats for 5 days under VI30 schedule for the seek link, followed by 3 days under VI60 schedule.
      5. For the VI30 schedule, allow the software to randomly select a time from a list of 15 s, 30 s, and 45 s, for an average of 30 s, whereas for the VI60 schedule, the software randomly selects a time from a list of 45 s, 60 s, and 75 s.
      6. Vary the inter-trial intervals along the different schedules of reinforcement. Start the inter-trial interval at 30 s during FR1 and VI5 but gradually increase to 7 min during VI30 and VI60.
  4. Punishment-imposed abstinence
    1. Start punishment sessions with insertion of seek lever and turning the houselight on. Allow punishment sessions for 8 days, with regular supervision to guarantee that animals do not show signs of abnormal pain or any health issues.
    2. Program the operant training system so that after completing the seek link under VI60, the seek lever retracts, the houselight is turned off, and a mild footshock is administered on 30% of the completed seek links instead of insertion of the take lever. The take lever extends on the other 70% of completed seek links, as in a normal self-administration trial.
    3. Program an inter-trial interval of 7 min with all cues and houselight off before inserting the seek lever and turning the houselight on to signal the beginning of the next trial.
    4. Set the footshock intensity at 0.2 mA and increase by 0.1 mA per punishment day until 1.0 mA.
    5. After rats press the take lever (on trials when it is available), repeat step 3.3.4.2 for retraction of the lever, delivery of the heroin infusion, and presentation of cues, followed by a 7 min inter-trial interval.
    6. Start the next trial with the insertion of the seek lever and turning the houselight on.
    7. If the rat does not press any lever for 10 min, follow step 3.3.5.5.
  5. Acute food deprivation (24 h)
    1. On the day following the last day of punishment-imposed abstinence, divide rats into two matched groups (according to body weight and the average number of seek lever and infusions over the last 3 days of self-administration training): Food Deprived (FD) and Sated.
    2. Remove food hoppers from the chambers (around 9:30 AM) for rats that undergo food deprivation-induced relapse test and keep food hoppers for the sated group that has unlimited access to food. Provide unlimited access to water in both groups and supervise animals to guarantee that they do not show any health problems during the food deprivation period.
  6. Food deprivation-induced heroin seeking tests
    1. The morning after the 24 h of food deprivation, perform a 3 h heroin-seeking test, under a VI60 schedule with an inter-trial interval of 7 min with no shock or heroin infusions being delivered. Return food hoppers to the food-deprived rats after the 3 h session.
    2. On the morning after the first heroin seeking test, remove food hoppers from the chambers of the rats that were sated on the first relapse test. Allow the rats that were previously food-deprived unlimited access to food (sated condition) before and during the test.
    3. Perform another 3 h heroin seeking test on the next day (after 24 h food deprivation), as in step 3.6.1.
    4. The day after the test euthanize the rats using a carbon dioxide chamber (100% CO2 at fill rate of 30-70% of the chamber volume per minute).

Results

Male rats demonstrated an increase in seek lever presses as the schedule of reinforcement increased over the training days, and a reliable, consistent number of heroin infusions over training days (Figure 1). During punishment, rats decreased the number of seek lever presses and infusions with the increase of footshock intensity over 8 punishment days (Figure 2). Food-deprivation condition significantly increased heroin seeking after punishment-imposed abstinenc...

Discussion

There are two important demonstrations in this paper. First, the validation of the punishment-imposed abstinence using the seek and take chain with heroin. Second, it was demonstrated that stress-induced relapse could be observed in a punishment-imposed abstinence procedure. These are important demonstrations because (i) The punishment-induced abstinence procedure more closely mimics the human condition as it results in voluntary abstinence, i.e., not due to extinction of drug-seeking or forced removal from the drug-taki...

Disclosures

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Natural Sciences & Engineering Council Discovery Program (US: RGPIN-2016-06694).

Materials

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
Anafen Injection 100 mg/mL Vial/50 mLMERIAL Canada, Inc.1938126anti-inflammatory drug
Balance armCoulbourn InstrumentsH29-01
Cannulae (22 G, 5-up)Plastics OneC313G-5up
Environment connection board & Linc cableCoulbourn InstrumentsH03-04
Fixed speed infusion pump (3.3 RPM)Coulbourn InstrumentsA73-01-3.3
GE Marine SiliconGESE-1134
Graphic State Notation 3Coulbourn InstrumentsGS3Software
Habitest universal LincCoulbourn InstrumentsH02-08
Heroin HClNational Institute for Drug Abuse, Research Triangle Park, NC, USA
House light-RatCoulbourn InstrumentsH11-01R
Isofluorane USP 99.9% Vial/250 mLFresenius Kabi Canada Ltd2237518
Liquid Swivels, Plastic, 22 GLomir Biomedical, Inc.RSP1
Rat test cageCoulbourn InstrumentsH10-11R-TCOperant conditioning chambers
Retractable lever-RatCoulbourn InstrumentsH23-17RA
Silastic tubing (ID 0.02, OD 0.037)Fisher Scientific (Canada)1118915A
Single high-bright cue-RatCoulbourn InstrumentsH11-03R
Sound attenuation boxesConcordia UniversityHome made
Stainless steal grid floorCoulbourn InstrumentsH10-11R-TC-SF
System controller 2Coulbourn InstrumentsSYS CTRL 2
System power baseCoulbourn InstrumentsH01-01
Tone module 2.9 KHzCoulbourn InstrumentsH12-02R-2.9
Tygon tubing (ID 0.02, OD 0.060)VWR63018-044

References

  1. Jaffe, J. H., Gilman, A. G., Rall, T. W., Nies, A. S., Taylor, P. Drug addiction and drug abuse. Goodman & Gilman's the Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. , 522-573 (1990).
  2. O'Brien, C. P., McLellan, A. T. Myths about the treatment of addiction. Lancet. 347, 237-240 (1996).
  3. Miczek, K. A., de Wit, H. Challenges for translational psychopharmacology research--some basic principles. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 199 (3), 291-301 (2008).
  4. Sinha, R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse. Psychopharmacology. 158, 343-359 (2001).
  5. Cheskin, L. J., Hess, J. M., Henningfield, J., Gorelick, D. A. Calorie restriction increases cigarette use in adult smokers. Psychopharmacology. 179, 430-436 (2005).
  6. Krahn, D., Kurth, C., Demitrack, M., Drewnowski, A. The relationship of dieting severity and bulimic behaviors to alcohol and other drug use in young women. Journal of Substance Abuse. 4, 341-353 (1992).
  7. Shalev, U., Highfield, D., Yap, J., Shaham, Y. Stress and relapse to drug seeking in rats: studies on the generality of the effect. Psychopharmacology. 150, 337-346 (2000).
  8. Peck, J. A., Ranaldi, R. Drug abstinence: exploring animal models and behavioral treatment strategies. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 231 (10), 2045-2058 (2014).
  9. Cooper, A., Barnea-Ygael, N., Levy, D., Shaham, Y., Zangen, A. A conflict rat model of cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking). Psychopharmacology (Berl). 194 (1), 117-125 (2007).
  10. Barnea-Ygael, N., Yadid, G., Yaka, R., Ben-Shahar, O., Zangen, A. Cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in the rat "conflict model": effect of prolonged home-cage confinement. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 219 (3), 875-883 (2012).
  11. Fredriksson, I., Applebey, S. V., Minier-Toribio, A., Shekara, A., Bossert, J. M., Shaham, Y. Effect of the dopamine stabilizer (-)-OSU6162 on potentiated incubation of opioid craving after electric barrier-induced voluntary abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 45 (5), 770-779 (2020).
  12. Pelloux, Y., Everitt, B. J., Dickinson, A. Compulsive drug seeking by rats under punishment: effects of drug taking history. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 194 (1), 127-137 (2007).
  13. Marchant, N. J., Campbell, E. J., Kaganovsky, K. Punishment of alcohol-reinforced responding in alcohol preferring P rats reveals a bimodal population: Implications for models of compulsive drug seeking. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 87, 68-77 (2018).
  14. Krasnova, I. N., et al. Incubation of methamphetamine and palatable food craving after punishment-induced abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 39 (8), 2008-2016 (2014).
  15. Panlilio, L. V., Thorndike, E. B., Schindler, C. W. Reinstatement of punishment-suppressed opioid self-administration in rats: an alternative model of relapse to drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 168 (1-2), 229-235 (2003).
  16. Economidou, D., Pelloux, Y., Robbins, T. W., Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J. High impulsivity predicts relapse to cocaine-seeking after punishment-induced abstinence. Biological Psychiatry. 65 (10), 851-856 (2009).
  17. Sedki, F., D'Cunha, T., Shalev, U. A procedure to study the effect of prolonged food restriction on heroin seeking in abstinent rats. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE. (81), e50751 (2013).
  18. Venniro, M., Caprioli, D., Shaham, Y. Animal models of drug relapse and craving: From drug priming-induced reinstatement to incubation of craving after voluntary abstinence. Progress in Brain Research. 224, 25-52 (2016).
  19. Deroche-Gamonet, V., Piazza, P. V. Psychobiology of cocaine addiction: Contribution of a multi-symptomatic animal model of loss of control. Neuropharmacology. 76, 437-449 (2014).
  20. Pelloux, Y., Everitt, B. J., Dickinson, A. Compulsive drug seeking by rats under punishment: effects of drug taking history. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 194 (1), 127-137 (2007).
  21. Epstein, D. H., Kowalczyk, W. J. Compulsive seekers: Our take. Two clinicians' perspective on a new animal model of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 43 (4), 677-679 (2018).
  22. Hser, Y. I., Hoffman, V., Grella, C. E., Anglin, M. D. A 33-year follow-up of narcotics addicts. Archives of General Psychiatry. 58 (5), 503-508 (2001).
  23. Chen, B. T., et al. Rescuing cocaine-induced prefrontal cortex hypoactivity prevents compulsive cocaine seeking. Nature. 496 (7445), 359-362 (2013).

Reprints and Permissions

Request permission to reuse the text or figures of this JoVE article

Request Permission

Explore More Articles

Stress induced RelapseHeroin SeekingPunishment imposed AbstinenceOperant ConditioningEcological ValidityAnimal ModelsSelf administrationLever PressFixed Ratio OneNeuronal MechanismsFood DeprivationTraining ProtocolIntertrial IntervalInfusion DoseBehavioral Study

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Privacy

Terms of Use

Policies

Research

Education

ABOUT JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved