JoVE Logo

Sign In

A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content. Sign in or start your free trial.

In This Article

  • Summary
  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Protocol
  • Results
  • Discussion
  • Disclosures
  • Acknowledgements
  • Materials
  • References
  • Reprints and Permissions

Summary

This protocol introduces lateralized early odor preference learning in rats using acute single naris occlusion. Lateralized learning permits the examination of behavioral outcomes and underpinning biological mechanisms within the same animals, reducing variance induced by between-animal designs. This protocol can be used to investigate molecular mechanisms underpinning early odor learning.

Abstract

Rat pups during a critical postnatal period (≤ 10 days) readily form a preference for an odor that is associated with stimuli mimicking maternal care. Such a preference memory can last from hours, to days, even life-long, depending on training parameters. Early odor preference learning provides us with a model in which the critical changes for a natural form of learning occur in the olfactory circuitry. An additional feature that makes it a powerful tool for the analysis of memory processes is that early odor preference learning can be lateralized via single naris occlusion within the critical period. This is due to the lack of mature anterior commissural connections of the olfactory hemispheres at this early age. This work outlines behavioral protocols for lateralized odor learning using nose plugs. Acute, reversible naris occlusion minimizes tissue and neuronal damages associated with long-term occlusion and more aggressive methods such as cauterization. The lateralized odor learning model permits within-animal comparison, therefore greatly reducing variance compared to between-animal designs. This method has been used successfully to probe the circuit changes in the olfactory system produced by training. Future directions include exploring molecular underpinnings of odor memory using this lateralized learning model; and correlating physiological change with memory strength and durations.

Introduction

Olfaction is the primary sensory modality in rodents, without which they would not be able to successfully navigate or survive in their environment. It is especially critical for neonatal pups, which can neither see nor hear during the first post-natal week, to use olfaction in order to locate their mother to feed1. As a result, neonatal rat pups can be conditioned to prefer odors with simple experimental manipulations. A variety of stimuli have been used as the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) to induce conditioned responses to novel odors (conditioned stimulus, CS) in neonates, including the nesting environment2,3, milk suckling4-6, stroking or tactile stimulation7-12, tail pinch13, maternal saliva13, mild foot shock14-18, and intracranial brain stimulation19. The present study employs a well-established early odor preference paradigm wherein an odor, in this case peppermint, is combined with tactile stimulation in order to produce a preference for peppermint 24 hr later10,11,20. These odors memories are dependent on intact olfactory circuitry, primarily including the olfactory bulbs (OB)21-23 and the anterior piriform cortex (aPC)24,25.

Experimental investigations of the early odor preference learning have deepened and broadened our understanding of the molecular and physiological underpinnings of a mammalian memory. This mammalian model has several advantages in studying memory mechanisms. First, the neural sources of the UCS signal have been identified. Various stimuli as mentioned above stimulate locus coeruleus norepinephrine release26, which in turn activates multiple adrenoceptors in the OB and aPC, causing cellular and physiological effects that support learning22,27,28. Second, memory-supporting mechanisms take place in well-defined laminar neural structures. The simplicity of the olfactory circuitry in neonatal rats provides researchers with the ideal framework with which to uncover the intricate processes related to synaptic plasticity. Olfactory sensory neurons (OSN) in the olfactory epithelium project onto mitral/tufted cells in the OB and these mitral/tufted cells in turn project ipsilaterally to piriform cortex (PC) via the lateral olfactory tract (LOT), among other structures29. Both the OSN synapses in the OB30,31 and the LOT synapses24,25 in aPC have been identified as critical loci for synaptic changes that support learning and memory. Third, in an early age in rats, olfactory inputs can readily be lateralized. Each aPC has access to bilateral odor information via the anterior commissure once this white matter is fully formed at post-natal day 12 (PD12)32. Before PD 12, odor input can be isolated to ipisilateral OB and aPC through single naris occlusion24,25,31,33,34. Single naris occlusion permits the odor memory formation from the open naris, and prevents the same memory from the occluded naris prior to PD 1233. Odor memory is isolated to the ipsilateral hemisphere including both OB and aPC. Therefore, each rat pup can be its own control for learning and underpinning physiology.

In the present study, the lateralized early odor preference learning protocol is introduced. This method serves as a powerful tool for studying neural mechanisms underpinning odor learning by providing an intra-animal control24,25,31 , thereby reducing both the number of animals required and the general variation. Naris occlusion is reversible in that the grease or nose plug can be applied and removed with minimal stress or damage to the animal. Here, first, detailed procedures of early odor preference training and testing are described, with a focus on the lateralized protocol using single naris occlusion with a nose plug. Then results are presented to demonstrate the effectiveness of single naris occlusion in isolating odor input and producing lateralized odor memory. Finally, the potentials of using this lateralized learning model to study physiological changes in the olfactory system that both generate learning and support memory expression are discussed.

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Protocol

Sprague Dawley rat (Charles River) pups of both genders are used. Litters are culled to 12 on PD1 (birth being PD0). The dams are maintained on a 12 hr light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. Experimental procedures have been approved by Memorial University’s Institutional Animal Care Committee.

1. Nose Plug Construction

NOTE: This procedure was adapted and modified from Cummings et al. (1997)35.

  1. Aquire polyethylene-20 tubing and 3-0 silk suture thread.
  2. Cut a small piece of polyethylene-20 tubing to approximately 0.8 mm.
  3. Thread silk suture through the prepared tubing such that there is thread on either side of the section of tubing.
  4. On one end of the thread outside of the plug, tie a knot in the thread.
  5. Pull the section of tubing down over the knot in the thread. The knot should lodge inside the tubing.
  6. Trim both ends of the thread such that ~2 mm of thread is protruding from one end of the tubing (see Figure 1A).

2. Naris Occlusion before Training

  1. Remove pup from dam and place in a secure dish covered with regular bedding.
  2. Use a cotton tip application to dab a local anesthetic jelly, 2% Xylocaine, on the naris to be occluded.
  3. Allow the pup to rest in the dish for ~3 min.
  4. Hold the pup gently but securely in the non-dominant hand.
  5. Using the dominant hand, pick up a nose plug and dab the same local anesthetic jelly around the tip from which the thread is not protruding. This will act as both an anesthetic for any minor pains associated with plug insertion and will act as a lubricant inside the naris. NOTE: The effect of Xylocaine starts within a couple of minutes and lasts 20-30 min. Pups in general show good tolerance for plug insertion after xylocaine jelly application (minimum struggling and vocalization).
  6. Gently insert the nose plug by firmly holding the pup and slowly rotating the plug with very gentle pushes until the plug is fully inserted and only the 2 mm thread is protruding from the naris (see Figure 1B). There should be no bleeding from either naris during this process. Pups with bleeding during nose plug insertion are excluded and returned to dams
  7. Allow the animal to rest in this dish for 5 min in order to habituate to the plug.
  8. Remove the pup from the habituation dish and begin the conditioning paradigm24.

figure-protocol-2695
Figure 1. Construction of a nose plug. A) Schematics showing the steps of making a nose plug. A thread is pulled through polyethylene tubing; a knot is made and pulled into the middle of the tubing to block it; two ends are cut with a 2 mm residue in one end out of the tubing. B) Front and lateral view of a rat with a nose plug in one naris.

3. Scented Bedding Preparation

  1. Wearing new gloves and in a fume hood to prevent odor contamination, place 500 ml of woodchip bedding into a plastic bag.
  2. Use a syringe to draw up 0.3 ml peppermint extract, and spray this over the bedding in the plastic bag.
  3. Tie the bag shut, shake the bag vigorously, and allow the bedding to rest in the bag for 5 min.
  4. Place the scented bedding in a clear, shallow, acrylic training box (20 x 20 x 5 cm3, Figure 2A) uncovered in a fume hood for 5 min before use. Once the bedding is prepared, discard these gloves, and do not allow these gloves to come in contact with the animals.
  5. Place the unscented bedding in an identical clear plastic box, and ensure that it does not come into contact with the scented bedding or used gloves.

4. Odor Conditioning Paradigm (See Picture in Figure 2A)

Pups undergo either a single conditioning session, on PD 6, or multiple trial sessions (one session per day, PD 3-6).

  1. Place the habituated pup on scented bedding. For control odor only (O/S-) pups, leave these pups on the bedding for 10 min, then skip to step 4.5. For experimental odor + stroke (O/S+) pups, continue to the following steps in this section.
  2. Stroke the pup for 30 sec using a small paintbrush. Use rapid circular motions primarily around the hind region of the pup.
  3. Allow the pup to rest for 30 sec.
  4. Repeat steps 4.2 & 4.3 for a total of 10 min (i.e. 10 pairings of stroking + odor).
  5. Remove pup from the conditioning box, remove the nose plug and return the pup to the dam.

5. Lateralized Odor Preference Testing (See Picture in Figure 2B)

Testing occurs at various time points (e.g., 24 or 48 hr) following the final training session. Testing is carried out in a stainless steel testing chamber (30 x 20 x 18 cm3), which is placed on top of two training boxes (training box is described in 3.4), separated by a 2 cm neutral zone. One training box contains peppermint-scented bedding while the other box contains clean, unscented bedding. The floor of the testing chamber is a metal grid, which is then covered by a removable sheet of plastic mesh (Figure 2B).

  1. Prepare one peppermint and one unscented bedding as per Section 3, and place each box under opposite sides of the testing chamber, 2 cm apart. Place the plastic mesh on the metal-grid floor of the testing chamber.
  2. Remove the pup from the dam and place a firm dab of odorless silicone grease on the naris that is occluded during training. Re-apply the grease throughout the first testing procedure as needed. NOTE: Random naris occlusion during training and testing may be considered to avoid bias.
  3. Place pup in the neutral zone of the testing chamber.
  4. Allow the pup to explore the chamber for 1 min, recording how long the pup spent over the two sides of the chamber (i.e. over peppermint or neutrally-scented bedding).
  5. Allow the pup to rest for 1 min in a covered plastic holding chamber.
  6. Repeat steps 5.2 & 5.3 for a total of 10 min (i.e. 5 test trials separated by 5 rest trials) switching the initial orientation of the pup in the chamber in order to control for direction preferences.
  7. Immediately following testing, wipe away the grease from the naris.
  8. Insert a polyethylene noseplug into the opposite naris as per section 2 and allow the animal to rest for 10 min.
  9. Test the pup once again as in 5.3 -5.6, remove the plug, and return the pup to the dam. Remove and clean the plastic mesh of the testing chamber with 95% ethanol and allow the liquid to evaporate. Place the mesh back before testing the next pup.
    NOTE: Applying silicone grease at the first naris occlusion during testing prevents the chance of bleeding and stress associated with nose plug insertion.

figure-protocol-7452
Figure 2. Early odor preference training and testing. A) Early odor preference training using odor + stroking paradigm. B) Two choice odor preference testing with peppermint bedding on one side, control unscented bedding on the opposite side. A 2 cm neutral zone is placed in between.

6. Testing the Effectiveness of Single Naris Occlusion

This experiment is performed to determine whether single naris occlusion leads to lateralized activation of the olfactory system.

  1. Perform unilateral naris occlusions on PD 6 or 7 pups as described in section 2.
  2. After ~5 min habituation, place the pup in a covered plastic container and expose it to 30 µl pure peppermint oil soaked in a piece of tissue for 10 min.
  3. Immediately after the peppermint odor exposure, inject the pup intraperitoneally (i.p.) with chloral hydrate (400 mg/kg) as a general anesthetic, or pentobarbitol, (150 mg/kg).
  4. Once fully anesthetized (showing no response to tail or foot pinch), transcardially perfuse the pup with ice-cold solutions of saline (0.9%) for ~1 min, followed by paraformaldehyde (4%, dissolved in 0.1 M phosphate buffer solution, PBS).
  5. After 10 min of paraformaldehyde perfusion, collect the brain and place it in paraformaldehyde overnight at 4 °C, then transfer the brain to a sucrose solution (20% in PBS) for an additional 24 hr.
  6. Cut coronal brain slices at 30 μm thickness with a cryostat. Collect OB and PC slices and mount onto gelatin-coated slides, followed by standard immunohistochemistry staining for pCREB antibody21,25,30.

7. Testing the Reversibility of Single Naris Occlusion

This experiment tests whether the blocking effect is reversible at 24 hr following the removal of the nose plug.

  1. Perform unilateral naris occlusions on PD 6 or 7 pups as described in section 2.
  2. After 15 min (equivalent to the duration of the naris occlusion during training – 5 min habituation + 10 min training), remove the nose plug, and return the pup to the dam.
  3. 24 hr later, expose the pup to the peppermint odor in a covered plastic container for 10 min as described in 6.2.
  4. Follow same steps in sections 6.3-6.6.

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Results

Here we review some of the previously established results24 to demonstrate the effectiveness of the naris occlusion in isolating odor input and learning to one hemisphere, and the reversibility of this method.

Single naris occlusion during early odor preference training leads to a lateralized odor memory24. The memory is confined to the spared naris (Figure 3). When pups are tested for odor preference with the same naris occluded as during training, they ...

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Discussion

The lateralized odor learning and memory model in rat pups within a critical time window was first established by Hall and colleagues. In a series of studies33,34,36, they showed that an odor preference memory could be lateralized by odor + milk pairings to one naris at PD 6 in rat pups. Preference memory was robust when the same naris was open during training and testing, but not observed when the occluded naris was unblocked and tested. However, at PD 12, when anterior commissural connections from the anteri...

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Disclosures

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a CIHR operating grant (MOP-102624) to Q. Y. We thank Dr Carolyn Harley for helpful discussions throughout the study, Dr. Qinlong Hou, Amin Shakhawat, and Andrea Darby-King for technical support.

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Materials

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
Polythylene 20 tubingIntramedic427406Non-radiopaque, Non-toxic
3-0 Silk suture threadSynetureSofsilkNon-absorbent
Silicone greaseWarner Instrument64-0378Odorless
2% Xylocaine gelAstraZenecaProd. No 061Lidocaine hydrochloride jelly, purchased at local pharmacy
Paint brushDynasty206RSimilar size/other brands work too
Peppermint extractSigma-AldrichW284807Other brands should be okay too
Training boxCustom-madeN/AAcrylic box (20 x 20 x 5 cm3), see Figure 2A. Parameters and material for the box are not critical and can be modified. Material used should be odorless and does not absorb odors
Testing chamberCustom-madeN/AStainless steel (30 x 20 x 18 cm3), see Figure 2B. Parameters and material for the chamber are not critical and can be modified. For example, an acrylic chamber instead of a stainless steel one can be used
pCREB antibodyCell Signaling9198Ser 133 (87G3) Rabbit mAb
Chloral hydrateSigma-AldrichC8383N/A
ParaformaldehypeSigma-AldrichP6148N/A
SucroseSigma-AldrichS9378N/A

References

  1. Gregory, E. H., Pfaff, D. W. Development of olfactory-guided behavior in infant rats. Physiol Behav. 6, 573-576 (1971).
  2. Alberts, J. R., May, B. Nonnutritive, thermotactile induction of filial huddling in rat pups. Dev Psychobiol. 17, 161-181 (1984).
  3. Galef, B. G., Kaner, H. C. Establishment and maintenance of preference for natural and artificial olfactory stimuli in juvenile rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 94, 588-595 (1980).
  4. Johanson, I. B., Hall, W. G. Appetitive learning in 1-day-old rat pups. Science. 205, 419-421 (1979).
  5. Johanson, I. B., Hall, W. G. Appetitive conditioning in neonatal rats: conditioned orientation to a novel odor. Dev Psychobiol. 15, 379-397 (1982).
  6. Johanson, I. B., Teicher, M. H. Classical conditioning of an odor preference in 3-day-old rats. Behav Neural Biol. 29, 132-136 (1980).
  7. McLean, J. H., Darby-King, A., Sullivan, R. M., King, S. R. Serotonergic influence on olfactory learning in the neonate rat. Behav Neural Biol. 60, 152-162 (1993).
  8. Moore, C. L., Power, K. L. Variation in maternal care and individual differences in play, exploration, and grooming of juvenile Norway rat offspring. Dev Psychobiol. 25, 165-182 (1992).
  9. Pedersen, P. E., Williams, C. L., Blass, E. M. Activation and odor conditioning of suckling behavior in 3-day-old albino rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 8, 329-341 (1982).
  10. Sullivan, R. M., Hall, W. G. Reinforcers in infancy: classical conditioning using stroking or intra-oral infusions of milk as UCS. Dev Psychobiol. 21, 215-223 (1988).
  11. Sullivan, R. M., Leon, M. Early olfactory learning induces an enhanced olfactory bulb response in young rats. Brain Res. 392, 278-282 (1986).
  12. Weldon, D. A., Travis, M. L., Kennedy, D. A. Posttraining D1 receptor blockade impairs odor conditioning in neonatal rats. Behav Neurosci. 105, 450-458 (1991).
  13. Sullivan, R. M., Hofer, M. A., Brake, S. C. Olfactory-guided orientation in neonatal rats is enhanced by a conditioned change in behavioral state. Dev Psychobiol. 19, 615-623 (1986).
  14. Camp, L. L., Rudy, J. W. Changes in the categorization of appetitive and aversive events during postnatal development of the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 21, 25-42 (1988).
  15. Moriceau, S., Wilson, D. A., Levine, S., Sullivan, R. M. Dual circuitry for odor-shock conditioning during infancy: corticosterone switches between fear and attraction via amygdala. J Neurosci. 26, 6737-6748 (2006).
  16. Roth, T. L., Sullivan, R. M. Endogenous opioids and their role in odor preference acquisition and consolidation following odor-shock conditioning in infant rats. Dev Psychobiol. 39, 188-198 (2001).
  17. Roth, T. L., Sullivan, R. M. Consolidation and expression of a shock-induced odor preference in rat pups is facilitated by opioids. Physiol Behav. 78, 135-142 (2003).
  18. Sullivan, R. M. Developing a sense of safety: the neurobiology of neonatal attachment. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1008, 122-131 (2003).
  19. Wilson, D. A., Sullivan, R. M. Olfactory associative conditioning in infant rats with brain stimulation as reward. I. Neurobehavioral consequences. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 53, 215-221 (1990).
  20. Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A., Leon, M. Associative Processes in Early Olfactory Preference Acquisition: Neural and Behavioral Consequences. Psychobiology. , 29-33 (1989).
  21. McLean, J. H., Harley, C. W., Darby-King, A., Yuan, Q. pCREB in the neonate rat olfactory bulb is selectively and transiently increased by odor preference-conditioned training. Learn Mem. 6, 608-618 (1999).
  22. Sullivan, R. M., Stackenwalt, G., Nasr, F., Lemon, C., Wilson, D. A. Association of an odor with activation of olfactory bulb noradrenergic beta-receptors or locus coeruleus stimulation is sufficient to produce learned approach responses to that odor in neonatal rats. Behav Neurosci. 114, 957-962 (2000).
  23. Yuan, Q., Harley, C. W., McLean, J. H. Mitral cell beta1 and 5-HT2A receptor colocalization and cAMP coregulation: a new model of norepinephrine-induced learning in the olfactory bulb. Learn Mem. 10, 5-15 (2003).
  24. Fontaine, C. J., Harley, C. W., Yuan, Q. Lateralized odor preference training in rat pups reveals an enhanced network response in anterior piriform cortex to olfactory input that parallels extended memory. J Neurosci. 33, 15126-15131 (2013).
  25. Morrison, G. L., Fontaine, C. J., Harley, C. W., Yuan, Q. A role for the anterior piriform cortex in early odor preference learning: evidence for multiple olfactory learning structures in the rat pup. J Neurophysiol. 110, 141-152 (2013).
  26. Nakamura, S., Kimura, F., Sakaguchi, T. Postnatal development of electrical activity in the locus ceruleus. J Neurophysiol. 58, 510-524 (1987).
  27. Harley, C. W., Darby-King, A., McCann, J., McLean, J. H. Beta1-adrenoceptor or alpha1-adrenoceptor activation initiates early odor preference learning in rat pups: support for the mitral cell/cAMP model of odor preference learning. Learn Mem. 13, 8-13 (2006).
  28. Shakhawat, A. M., Harley, C. W., Yuan, Q. Olfactory bulb alpha2-adrenoceptor activation promotes rat pup odor-preference learning via a cAMP-independent mechanism. Learn Mem. 19, 499-502 (2012).
  29. Isaacson, J. S. Odor representations in mammalian cortical circuits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 20, 328-331 (2010).
  30. Lethbridge, R., Hou, Q., Harley, C. W., Yuan, Q. Olfactory bulb glomerular NMDA receptors mediate olfactory nerve potentiation and odor preference learning in the neonate rat. PLoS One. 7, e35024 (2012).
  31. Yuan, Q., Harley, C. W. What a nostril knows: olfactory nerve-evoked AMPA responses increase while NMDA responses decrease at 24-h post-training for lateralized odor preference memory in neonate rat. Learn Mem. 19, 50-53 (2012).
  32. Schwob, J. E., Price, J. L. The development of axonal connections in the central olfactory system of rats. J Comp Neurol. 223, 177-202 (1984).
  33. Kucharski, D., Hall, W. G. New routes to early memories. Science. 238, 786-788 (1987).
  34. Kucharski, D., Johanson, I. B., Hall, W. G. Unilateral olfactory conditioning in 6-day-old rat pups. Behav Neural Biol. 46, 472-490 (1986).
  35. Cummings, D. M., Henning, H. E., Brunjes, P. C. Olfactory bulb recovery after early sensory deprivation. J Neurosci. 17, 7433-7440 (1997).
  36. Kucharski, D., Hall, W. G. Developmental change in the access to olfactory memories. Behav Neurosci. 102, 340-348 (1988).
  37. Brunjes, P. C. Unilateral odor deprivation: time course of changes in laminar volume. Brain Res Bull. 14, 233-237 (1985).
  38. Kass, M. D., Pottackal, J., Turkel, D. J., McGann, J. P. Changes in the neural representation of odorants after olfactory deprivation in the adult mouse olfactory bulb. Chem Senses. 38, 77-89 (2013).
  39. Kim, H. H., Puche, A. C., Margolis, F. L. Odorant deprivation reversibly modulates transsynaptic changes in the NR2B-mediated CREB pathway in mouse piriform cortex. J Neurosci. 26, 9548-9559 (2006).
  40. Korol, D. L., Brunjes, P. C. Rapid changes in 2-deoxyglucose uptake and amino acid incorporation following unilateral odor deprivation: a laminar analysis. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 52, 75-84 (1990).
  41. Leung, C. H., Wilson, D. A. Trans-neuronal regulation of cortical apoptosis in the adult rat olfactory system. Brain Res. 984, 182-188 (2003).

Access restricted. Please log in or start a trial to view this content.

Reprints and Permissions

Request permission to reuse the text or figures of this JoVE article

Request Permission

Explore More Articles

Keywords Lateralized Odor LearningNeonatal RatsOlfactory CircuitryMemory FormationNaris OcclusionWithin animal ComparisonOlfactory SystemOdor Memory

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Privacy

Terms of Use

Policies

Research

Education

ABOUT JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved